Autonomous Motivation: The Key To Employee Performance and Workplace Success?
Autonomous Motivation: The Key To Employee Performance and Workplace Success?
Success?”
200527752
2012/13
“Gina Koutsopoulou”
BSc Psychology
I would like to thank my Supervisor Gina Koutsopoulou for her dedicated support and
guidance. Gina continuously provided encouragement and was always willing and
enthusiastic to assist in any way she could throughout the research project.
I would also like to thank Andrew Prestwich for providing advice regarding
mediational analysis.
Finally, many thanks to all participants that took part in the Study and enabled this
research to be possible.
I
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
ABSTRACT
In the face of modern pressures, competition and demands, with change and
uncertainty looming over the workplace there is perhaps no better time to uncover an
innovative motivational technique. The promotion of autonomy-support offers a
promising intervention to enhance high-quality forms of motivation and performance,
whilst maintaining employee wellbeing and quality of working life. Based on the Self-
Determination Theory, the present research aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of
an autonomy-supportive motivational style on intrinsic motivation and performance in
a learning task. The study employed an independent measures research design with
two experimental conditions that enabled a comparison between ‘autonomy-
supporting’ and ‘autonomy-thwarting’ motivational techniques. The research was
conducted with University Students to ascertain the utility of this motivational tool for
the next generation of workers. It was hypothesised that, compared to a condition
which incorporated a controlling motivational style, participants in an autonomy-
supportive condition would have greater intrinsic motivation (H1) and display superior
performance (H2). Moreover, it was hypothesised (H3) that a causal chain would be
observed in which autonomy-support affects intrinsic motivation which, in turn,
influences performance. All three hypotheses were found to be supported. The
results demonstrate consistent differences between the experimental conditions
despite the brief nature of the interaction, indicating the profound impact of
manipulating the perception of autonomy. Furthermore, they highlight that there is
more to motivation than simply its quantity and that an understanding of the quality of
motivation would be invaluable for various life domains. Finally, this study
demonstrates the simplicity with which perceived autonomy can be influenced and
presents a novel motivational tool that could enrich employee motivation, enhance
performance and drive business success.
II
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS…………………………………………………………………….…I
ABSTRACT…………………………………………………………………………………....II
TABLE OF CONTENTS…………………………………………………………………….III-IV
1. INTRODUCTION………………………………………………………………………….1-11
2. METHOD…….…………………………………………………………………………12-16
2.1. Participants………………………………………………………………………………..........12
2.2. Ethics……………………………………………………………………………………………..12
2.3. Materials……………………………………………………………………………………...12-14
2.5. Procedure…………………………………………………………………………………….15-16
3. RESULTS…….………………………………………………………………………...17-21
3.1. Manipulation Check……..……………………………………………………………………...17
3.3. ANOVAs………………………………………………………………………………………....18
3.6. Regression………………………………………………………………………………………20
4. DISCUSSION….………………………………………………………………………..22-33
4.1. Current Findings……..………………………………………………………………………22-24
4.5. Conclusion…………………………………………………………………………………….....33
III
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
REFERENCES……………………………………………………………………………...V-XIV
APPENDIX………………………………………………………………………………XV-XLVI
1. Reeve & Jang (2006) Instructional Behaviours..………………………………………….....XV
3. Power Calculations……………….……………………………………………………………XVII
Figure 3. Standardized regression coefficients between Condition and IM, mediated by PAS……….20
Figure 4. Standardized regression coefficients between PAS and Performance, mediated by IM…..21
IV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
1. INTRODUCTION
“In today's business environment, competition arises when other organisations seek
to do what your company does, only better” (Wagner & Hollenbeck, 2010, p.xviii).
Organisations are in continuous competition to uncover the latest strategy for
business success, whilst adapting to the unpredictable nature of the modern
workplace. The aim is to identify flexible and innovative solutions to maximise
business productivity without damaging employee wellbeing and quality of working
life (Jones et al. 2006). However, globalisation, the economic recession,
technological advancements and the ‘24/7 service culture’ are all playing a
prominent role in designing a damaging workplace. This is an environment in which
employees are working for longer, under increased pressure and demand, facing job
insecurity and diminished work-life balance (Kodz et al. 2002), which in turn has led
to increasing work-stress and stress-related illness (Jones & Bright, 2001). As a
result, Organisations are arguably becoming increasingly unaware of their key
competitive advantage; their employees. The recognition of employees as a valuable
asset calls for identification of the best technique to create an environment in which
talent can flourish and through which a competitive advantage can be created and
sustained.
1
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
2
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
The Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985a; Ryan & Deci, 2000) is a
prominent example of a theory that emphasises the importance of motivation quality,
in addition to its quantity. It builds upon the notion that an external or internal locus of
causality (deCharms, 1968) is influential in determining motivation. Specifically, SDT
proposes that there is a qualitative distinction between types of motivation that are
‘autonomous’ (internal locus of causality) and types of motivation that are ‘controlled’
(external locus of causality). Studies have been conducted in multiple domains that
emphasise autonomous motivation as being more effective than controlled
motivations with respect to various important outcomes, including learning,
performing effectively and behaving more healthily (Deci & Ryan, 2012). In relation
to the workplace, autonomous motivation has been positively related to increased
job satisfaction and engagement (Richer et al., 2002), job performance (Bono &
Judge, 2004) and organisational commitment (Lam & Gurland, 2008). Van den
Broeck et al. (2011) drew on SDT to understand the two main components of
workaholism (i.e. working excessively and working compulsively) and how they
relate to vigour and exhaustion. They found that autonomous motivation was
associated with excessive work, which related positively with vigour, whereas
controlled motivation related positively with compulsive work and exhaustion. These
findings clearly highlight the importance of considering the quality of motivation,
rather than simply the overall quantity, to ensure that motivational techniques are
supporting employee wellbeing in addition to promoting performance. As a whole,
these studies highlight the potential value of understanding and utilising the
underlying concepts of SDT in the workplace.
SDT is based on the proposition that all human beings have fundamental needs to
be autonomous, competent and related to others. The theory proposes that
3
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
4
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
al., 2009). This model highlights the distinction made by the CET by proposing that
intrinsic motivation, not extrinsic motivation, is a mediator of the relationship between
autonomy-support and performance-related outcomes. Kuvaas (2009) found partial
evidence, across a broad cross-section of job types, to support the proposition that
intrinsic motivation may mediate the relationship between autonomy-support and
ultimate work performance. This evidence suggests that promotion of intrinsic
motivation is essential to obtain desired performance outcomes.
SDT broadens the concept of intrinsic motivation using a final underlying theory; the
Organismic Integration Theory. This theory explains a process through which
extrinsically motivated behaviour can become intrinsic to the individual when
endorsed by significant others. This is a process of internalisation and, based on the
level of internalisation, four types of extrinsic motivation have been identified (Ryan
et al., 1985). External regulation is the most controlled form of motivation, followed
by introjected regulation which refers to an individual performing a behaviour to avoid
negative feelings. The third type is identified regulation which is motivated by the
valued outcomes of performing a behaviour. Finally, the most developed form of
extrinsically motivated behaviour is integrated motivations, which result from
behaviours that are considered to satisfy personal goals that are consistent with an
individual’s self-identity (Maltby et al. 2010). SDT proposes that it is the degree to
which the need for autonomy is satisfied that distinguishes whether identification or
integration, rather than merely introjection, will occur (Gagné & Deci, 2005). These
two developed forms of extrinsically motivated behaviour are classed as autonomous
because individuals understand and accept the personal value of the activity (Deci &
Ryan, 2012). Autonomous forms of extrinsic motivation are thought to be similar, in
the way that they operate, to intrinsically motivated behaviour (Maltby et al., 2010).
Therefore, SDT moves away from the intrinsic/extrinsic distinction of motivation,
towards distinguishing between autonomous and controlled motivations. Throughout
SDT it becomes evident that motivation is not simply a personally regulated state,
but conversely various social-contextual factors can substantially influence
motivation quality. This is achieved through satisfaction or thwarting of the basic
needs, influencing an individual’s causality orientation, enhancing or undermining
intrinsic motivation or affecting the internalisation process. In relation to the
workplace, this theme of SDT, in conjunction with its emphasis on the importance of
5
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
SDT claims that individuals are naturally inclined to seek contexts that support their
basic psychological needs (Sheldon & Grunz, 2009). Therefore, it becomes of
considerable importance for organisations to identify aspects of the work
environment that can support employees’ needs for autonomy, competence and
relatedness. Job autonomy is conceptualised as the extent to which a job provides
an individual with independence, freedom and discretion to make decisions over the
performance of certain roles and tasks (Morgeson & Humphrey, 2006). Therefore, an
autonomy-supportive work environment is a context that provides choice,
encourages self-initiation and acknowledges a subordinate’s perspective (Ryan &
Deci, 2000). Autonomy-supportive contexts are clearly associated with the
satisfaction of the need for autonomy; however these contexts have also been
associated with satisfying the needs for competence and relatedness (Deci & Ryan,
2012), which together improve psychological wellbeing and development.
Furthermore, autonomy-supportive environments have been associated with the
maintenance or enhancement of intrinsic motivation and in facilitating the
internalisation of external contingencies, while controlling contexts have been
associated with undermining intrinsic motivation and preventing internalisation (Black
& Deci, 2000). In relation to the workplace, an autonomy-supportive environment has
been associated with various positive health and organisational outcomes.
Specifically, a recent meta-analysis conducted by Humphrey et al. (2007)
demonstrated the salient role of perceived job autonomy in improving work
performance, satisfaction and commitment to the Organisation, in addition to
highlighting its negative association with absenteeism, stress and burnout. This
evidence indicates the value of providing autonomy-support in the workplace and
consequently begs the question of how autonomy can be supported. Gagné & Deci
(2005) claim that autonomy ‘supports’ fall into two categories, firstly, specific content
of a job, such as choice, can be adapted, and secondly, the general interpersonal
ambience can be improved which relates to organisational climate and manager or
6
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
leader interpersonal styles. Ryan & Deci (2006) emphasise it is not simply the
number of options provided to an individual that stimulates the perception of
autonomy, but rather it the nature of the support gained and the overall perception of
a culture or context as autonomous. Consequently, to obtain the greatest benefit
from an autonomy-supportive work environment, organisations need to consider the
organisational climate and specifically focus on enhancing key interpersonal
relationships. Therefore, an autonomy-supportive motivational style could be utilised
as a valuable technique to enhance interpersonal style and, in turn, have a profound
influence on employee motivation, wellbeing and performance.
7
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
8
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Modern Organisations are increasingly reliant on rapid and skilful innovation at all
levels of the business (Arnold et al., 2005), placing new pressures and expectations
on young workers joining the workforce. However, early work experiences have been
proposed to significantly shape employees’ subsequent work-related attitudes,
values and behaviours (Loughlin & Barling, 2001). Therefore, it is extremely
important for wellbeing and career development, that young employees are
supported. Moreover, as the workplace provides an extremely different context to
University life and school classrooms, young workers may require additional
constructive forms of support that will ensure the retention of motivation and
successful performance. It has been found that trainees learn and perform best
when they are autonomously engaged (Kozlowski et al., 2010) and when they have
access to autonomy-supportive mentors and autonomy-supportive organisational
climates (Bell & Kozlowski, 2002). Furthermore, evidence has shown that positive
forms of workplace initiative (Grant et al., 2011) and creativity (Hon, 2012) ensue
when employees are autonomously motivated, both of which are desired
characteristics of young employees entering the workforce (Bindl & Parker, 2009).
As a whole, this evidence suggests that autonomy-supportive management and
organisational climates would be beneficial to an organisation in regard to work
quality and productivity by enhancing valuable work-related outcomes in young
workers. However, generally there is limited research exploring the effects of
autonomy-supportive interventions on University-aged students, with the vast
majority of them being conducted with younger children in a teaching or coaching
context (McLachlan & Hagger, 2010). An understanding of the effectiveness of this
motivational technique on the next generation of workers would be of considerable
importance to organisations that are looking for new strategies to stimulate their
young employees.
Intrinsic motivation has become increasingly necessary for life success, particularly
in relation to higher education and the workplace (Pulfrey et al., 2013). It is
considered a high-quality form of motivation that is associated with individuals being
more persistent and self-driven (Deci & Ryan, 2000), more active in response to
9
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
autonomy-support and more successful in achieving goals (Gagné & Deci, 2005).
This suggests that identifying methods that can promote and enhance intrinsic
motivation could be invaluable in various life domains. Self-Determination Theory
proposes that through facilitating the satisfaction of basic psychological needs, social
contexts can impact the quality of an individual’s motivation. The promotion of
autonomy-support in specific social contexts offers a promising intervention to
enhance intrinsic motivation through need satisfaction. However, few studies have
attempted to experimentally manipulate the perception of autonomy (Pavey &
Sparks, 2012). Nevertheless, based on previous research, it is reasonable to
suggest that through modifying an instructors’ motivational style to become more
autonomy-supportive, it could lead to satisfaction of fundamental psychological
needs, facilitation of autonomous motivation and promotion of effective performance.
Furthermore, according to the motivation mediation model (Jang et al., 2009) it is
possible that these advantageous personal and performance outcomes may be
mediated by an individual’s stimulated level of intrinsic motivation.
10
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
The Study proposed three hypotheses in accordance with the above rationale;
11
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
2. METHOD
2.1. PARTICIPANTS
The participant sample (N=70) was comprised of female (n=63) and male (n=7)
Undergraduate Psychology Students. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 25 years
(M=19.31, SD=1.136). Students voluntarily signed up to the Study with the incentive
of receiving participation credits. Participants were alternatively assigned to one of
two experimental conditions in which they completed a comprehension task and a
questionnaire. Both conditions were completed within 45 minutes.
2.2. ETHICS
This research met the current British Psychological Society ethical standards. Ethical
approval was granted by the Ethics committee of the Institute of Psychological
Sciences, Leeds University (see Appendix 19.). Participants were made aware of the
voluntary nature of their participation and their right to withdraw at any time and
without giving a reason. All participants completed a consent form (see Appendix
14.), confirming they had understood the ethical principles underlying the research.
All participants were fully debriefed. Data collected was recorded anonymously.
2.3. MEASURES
Intrinsic Motivation. The Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) (Ryan, 1982) was used
as a multidimensional measurement tool to assess participants’ subjective
experience of the task. McAuley et al. (1989) found strong support for the validity of
the IMI. It consists of 27 items to assess intrinsic motivation; however, shorter
versions have been confirmed to be reliable (McAuley et al., 1989). This study used
a 24-item version of the IMI (see Appendix 11.). Participants rated each item on a 6-
12
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Effort. Effort is positively related to intrinsic motivation. 4 items were used to assess
this construct. Statements such as “I tried very hard on this activity” were
used. These items had an alpha reliability of .90. See Appendix 11., items
labelled EF1-4.
13
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
14
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
2.5. PROCEDURE
Participants were provided with an information sheet (see Appendix 13.) and consent
form which highlighted the ethical nature of the study. The participants were told the
study was investigating individual differences in learning styles and memory. The
verbal instructions given to participants throughout the study varied according to
Condition (see above).
The first part of the study involved a reading task. All participants read the same text
on computer use in young children. However, in the AS condition participants chose
from two layouts of the passage (See Appendices 7-8.). Paper and pens were
provided. In the AS condition these resources were highlighted verbally and in the
written instructions as possible aids for learning. In the C condition the availability of
these resources was only highlighted through the written instructions. The Pilot
Study (N=12) carried out, indicated that 10 minutes was sufficient time for
Participants to have confidently read the passage. Participants in the AS condition
were given the option to choose to finish before the 10 minutes. During this task the
instructor left the room. On completion of the task participants (in both conditions)
were asked to sit silently for a few minutes before the next task began. This period of
inactivity was to allow time away from the text before a recognition task was
completed. Ryan et al. (1990) used a similar period of inactivity in their study prior to
a recall task and found 4 minutes to be sufficient. The Pilot Study also demonstrated
4 minutes was sufficient. Participants were then given a comprehension task,
consisting of 10 MCQs based on the passage. The Pilot study found 8 minutes to be
optimal time to complete the test. Participants in the AS condition were given the
option to finish before the allocated time. The final task was a questionnaire. All
participants were provided with an envelope to conceal the completed questionnaire.
This measure was intended to reduce social desirability and self-reporting bias. The
instructor also left the room whilst the questionnaire was being completed. All
participants were given a debrief sheet (see Appendix 15.) and verbally debriefed.
15
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
In addition to influencing timing, the Pilot Study highlighted errors in the written
content of the questionnaire which were corrected. It also led to the incorporation of
a space for additional comments and two demographic questions into the
questionnaire (see Appendix 12.).
16
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
3. RESULTS
This section will present statistical analysis of the experimental data. The criterion for
statistical significance was set at the alpha level .05. The data has been screened to
check statistical assumptions and found them to be satisfied.
Descriptive statistics obtained for both conditions are presented in Table 1 (see
below). This table presents the Means and Standards Deviations for Intrinsic
Motivation (IM; DV1) and Performance (DV2). It also presents the same descriptive
statistics for each subcomponent of the IM construct.
17
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Post-inferential analysis was carried out separately for each dependent variable (see
Appendices 2-3.). Cohen’s (1992) standard conventions were used to interpret effect
size and a value of .8 was set as the criterion for a good level of power. Post-
inferential analysis of IM (DV1) found a large effect size for the difference between
the two conditions, d=1.55. Power calculations for DV1 were found to be significant at
the alpha level .01 indicating a 99% probability of detecting a real effect, δ=6.48. A
medium effect size was obtained for Performance (DV2), d=0.57. The power
calculations obtained for DV2 found a 67% probability of detecting a real effect at the
alpha level .05, δ=2.38.
18
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
To explore the data further, three correlational analyses were conducted to assess
the relations between PAS, DV1 and DV2, when the data was collapsed across
conditions.
A correlation carried out demonstrated that PAS and Performance were not
significantly related (r=.190, n=70, p>.05, two tails) (see Appendix 4. for a scatterplot
illustrating this relationship).
19
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
3.6. REGRESSION
The amount of variance in DV1 explained by PAS and the subsequent effect of DV1
on the variance found in DV2 was examined using two simple regressions.
The first regression found PAS accounted for considerable variance in DV1; R2=.443.
This suggests that PAS accounts for 43.3% of the variance in IM. The second
regression conducted found IM accounted for 11.1% (R2=.111) of the variance in
Performance (DV2).
[1] The relationship between Condition (IV) and IM (DV1) was found to be mediated
by PAS. As Figure 3. (below) illustrates, the standardized regression coefficient
between the Condition and IM decreased substantially when controlling for PAS. The
other conditions necessary for a mediation, according to Baron & Kenny (1986),
were also met: Condition was a significant predictor of IM and of PAS, and PAS was
a significant predictor of IM.
PAS
-3.71** -.20**
Condition IM
-.80**
(-.34)
** p<.001
Figure 3. Standardized regression coefficients between
Condition and Intrinsic Motivation (IM) as mediated by
Perceived Autonomy-support (PAS)
[2] Although the conditions of mediation were met for a relationship between
Condition and Performance (DV2) as mediated by IM (DV1), the computed
standardized regression coefficients found a null mediation.
[3] Regression analysis shows PAS is not significantly related to performance, thus
not meeting the preconditions set by Baron & Kenny (1986) for a mediation.
20
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
IM
.20** 7.77*
PAS Performance
1.32 (-.40)
*p<.01 ** p<.001
21
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
4. DISCUSSION
The Self-Determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985a; Ryan & Deci, 2000) has led to
a wealth of research into the benefits associated with an autonomy-supportive social
context. Numerous studies have identified its ability to facilitate the satisfaction of
fundamental psychological needs, promote psychological wellbeing, enhance
autonomous motivation and lead to more effective performance in various life
domains. This Study aimed to assess the impact of an autonomy-supportive
motivational style, in contrast to a controlling style, on intrinsic motivation and
subsequent performance of University Students. Specifically, it assessed the
influence of an instructor’s motivational style in a brief 30 minute learning task. It was
hypothesised that, compared to a condition which incorporated a controlling
motivational style, Participants in an autonomy-supportive condition would have
greater intrinsic motivation (H1) and display superior performance (H2). Moreover, it
was hypothesised (H3) that a causal chain would be observed in which autonomy-
support affects intrinsic motivation which, in turn, influences performance.
The manipulation check demonstrates that across the two conditions Participants’
perception of autonomy-support provided by the instructor varied significantly in the
expected pattern. This implies that the incorporation of Reeve & Jang’s (2006)
‘autonomy-supports’ and ‘autonomy-thwarts’ into the autonomy-supportive and
controlling conditions, respectively, influenced a considerable difference in the
instructor’s motivational style.
The descriptive statistics indicated the expected pattern of results, with the
autonomy-supportive condition demonstrating higher Intrinsic Motivation (IM) and
Performance than the controlling condition. Inferential analysis confirmed these
results and provided support for Hypotheses 1 and 2. The analysis revealed that
between the two conditions there was a significant difference in IM and Performance,
with the finding being stronger for IM than for Performance. In combination with the
descriptive statistics, these results therefore support the expectation that IM would
be higher in the autonomy-supportive condition, in contrast to the controlling
22
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
condition (H1) and that Performance would follow the same pattern (H2). The
strength of the results relating to IM are perhaps more impressive when the analysis
of each subcomponent of the construct is considered. In particular, the difference
between the conditions in overall IM was highly significant despite the finding that the
subcomponent of ‘Tension’ was insignificant. This insignificance may relate to the
nature of the task, in that pressure was not induced in either condition as there was
no expected consequence for task performance. These results imply that the
difference in the instructor’s motivational style between the two experimental
conditions may have caused the distinct pattern of IM and Performance observed in
each condition. This therefore demonstrates the vast difference in motivation and
performance that can arise as a result of an individual experiencing autonomy or
feeling controlled.
23
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
accounted for 43.3% of the variance in IM, suggesting PAS can vastly impact high-
quality forms of motivation. IM was found to account for only 11.1% of the variance in
Performance. Whilst this finding implies a definite role for IM in learning
performance, it also suggests that other factors may be influential in the relationship.
Mediation analysis attempted to uncover a causal chain within the results to explain
how the experimental manipulation influenced the outcome variables. PAS was
found to mediate the relationship between the experimental condition and IM. This
indicates that an individual’s perception of autonomy is heavily influential in
translating autonomy-support into increases in IM. Although IM was not found to
mediate the relationship between the experimental manipulation and performance, it
was found to be a mediator between PAS and Performance. As a whole, these
results appear to support Hypothesis 3 by indicating a causal chain in which the
experimental condition, through PAS, influenced IM, which in turn influenced
performance in the learning task. This finding is consistent with the motivation
mediation model (Jang et al., 2009), which proposes that the relationship between
autonomy-support and performance is mediated by IM.
The present Study supports previous literature which has highlighted the applicability
of SDT to enhancing motivation and performance. It emphasises the profound
influence that an individual’s social context can have through facilitating autonomous
forms of motivation. Specifically, it was found that through experimentally
manipulating feelings of autonomy, intrinsic motivation can be enhanced which
subsequently facilitated performance. Furthermore, the study highlights the simplicity
with which perceived autonomy can be influenced and, in turn, has demonstrated
that enhancing high-quality forms of motivation is not necessarily a complicated and
effortful endeavour. As a whole, it proposes the necessity for figures of authority in
various life domains to understand the distinction between controlling and autonomy-
supportive motivational styles.
24
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
This study has revealed the profound influence an instructor’s motivational style can
have within a brief and simple interaction. This is of relevance to various real-life
domains in which, for example, extensive contact with a manager, teacher or coach
may not be possible but can still have a lasting motivational influence. In addition,
this finding is also of importance for managers in the modern workplace that are
striving to identify a ‘quick-fix’ to employee motivation and productivity. It suggests an
alternative motivational tool in place of potentially damaging controlling forms of
motivation, such as rewards and goal-setting. It is important to highlight that research
25
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
26
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Although undertaken within a learning instructor-based context, this study has great
applicability to the workplace. It specifically emphasises the potential for using a
manager’s motivational style as a tool for promoting high-quality forms of motivation.
The adoption of autonomy-support within manager-employee interactions could have
a lasting impact on a range of positive workplace outcomes, relating to employee
productivity, wellbeing and satisfaction (Baard et al., 2004). There is some debate
within the literature as to whether management style is malleable. Though this study
does not provide direct evidence that management style can be permanently
modified, it does imply that certain strategies or skills can be adopted that can shape
a motivational style to become more autonomy-supportive. Whether this will
eventually lead to an adaptation in a manager’s motivational style is certainly an
avenue for further study. However, the importance of targeting interventions at high
managerial levels becomes clear on consideration of the influence a manger can
have throughout an organisation. Williams & Deci (1996) found that facilitating
autonomous motivation through autonomy-support can influence an individual’s
subsequent use of an autonomy-supportive style and Moreau & Mageau (2012)
found that colleague autonomy-support can be influential in various work-related
outcomes. Consequently, starting a chain of autonomy-support at managerial levels
could lead to the production of an autonomy-supportive culture that runs throughout
an organisation.
27
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
28
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
and uncertainty looming over the workplace there is perhaps no better time to
promote a new autonomy-supportive motivational technique.
The participants recruited for this study were University Students and predominantly
female. Although research with this population has been highlighted as important in
relation to new workers joining the workforce, its direct applicability to the workplace
is limited through the nature of the task and use of an unknown instructor rather than
a known manager or supervisor. Furthermore, the use of predominantly female
participants may influence the interpretation and generalisability of these results.
Baard et al. (2004) found a consistent pattern of gender differences in the workplace,
with women generally perceiving their managers as less autonomy-supportive. In a
related vein, Tripathi (2011) highlights the need to consider cultural differences in
relation to need for autonomy-support when applying motivational strategies.
Moreover, they found this cultural variability was not obtained when using self-report
measures. In relation to this study, this finding cautions the sensitivity of the self-
report measurements that assessed motivation and perceived autonomy-support.
However, the use of an envelope to conceal participants’ questionnaires and reduce
social desirability may dampen this caution.
29
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
especially pronounced for this task. It has been suggested that autonomous-extrinsic
motivation is more predictive than intrinsic motivation for behaviours that are effort-
driven, relatively uninteresting and require discipline (Koestner & Losier, 2002).
Therefore, this limitation emphasises the need for this study to have measured
motivation type, and specifically the process of internalisation. Use of a Self-
Regulation Questionnaire (Ryan & Connell, 1989) to determine the type of motivation
that led to performance would have provided greater explanatory power.
Another limitation of this study is that Causality Orientation was not taken into
consideration. Causality orientation is an individual difference factor that refers to the
degree to which individual’s tend to self-regulate and be autonomous, controlled or
impersonally motivated. In various domains, causality orientation has been found to
independently predict performance, in addition to that predicted by autonomy-
support (Baard et al. 2004; Black & Deci, 2000; Ng et al., 2012). It is possible that
causality orientation may have affected performance in this study. Failure to include
a measure of causality orientation may explain why the performance outcome is
relatively unaccounted for by the variables measured. The General Causality
Orientation Scale (Deci &Ryan, 1985b) could have been used to assess causality
orientation in combination with an autonomy-supportive motivational style.
Alternatively, Causality Orientation should have been assessed separately to remove
its effects in order to solely measure the influence of autonomy-support on
motivation and performance. Finally, individual differences in natural ability related to
the task should have been considered.
30
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Another avenue for future research is applying this experimental design, or a design
applicable to the workplace, that will include an assessment of need satisfaction.
This study has utilised the SDT to identify a path through which motivation and
performance can be enhanced. However, if the specific autonomy-supports utilised
in this study were also found to directly satisfy basic human needs, this motivational
technique could be used to enhance both performance and wellbeing. In relation to
the workplace, if concrete managerial behaviours could be identified that enhance
motivation and simultaneously facilitate performance and wellbeing, an organisation
could gain positive recognition for being both successful and employee-focussed.
Recently, the importance of satisfying all three psychological needs (i.e. autonomy,
relatedness, competence) has been stressed (Sheldon & Niemiec, 2006). Although
Deci & Ryan (2012) claimed that an autonomy-supportive environment could support
the satisfaction of all three needs, it may be beneficial to incorporate into autonomy-
support interventions factors that could specifically promote competence and
relatedness. Use of goal-setting, rewards and feedback are frequently used
controlling forms of motivation in the workplace. However, as highlighted previously,
they could be used positively within an autonomy-supportive environment to facilitate
satisfaction of the competence need. The need for relatedness could be satisfied by
incorporating means to create a positive leader-member exchange between
managers and their employees. Graves & Luciano (2013) have demonstrated that a
high-quality leader-member exchange facilitates employee self-determination, thus
indicating the potential for positive relationships to enhance need satisfaction. Future
research to assess the best individual factors that can be incorporated into an SDT-
based intervention to satisfy all or specific needs would be invaluable to target
interventions to specific organisational or employee requirements.
31
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
outcomes (Moran et al., 2012). Therefore, future research should take a ‘person-
centred’ approach to investigate the effect of autonomy-supportive motivational
strategies on different motivation ‘types’ and the subsequent outcomes. Interventions
could subsequently use profiling to adapt certain components to become more
focused to employees that are within a certain ‘cluster’. Moreover, this can facilitate
identification of individuals that are most in need of autonomy-support, and then
measures can be taken to ensure their support is increased. For example, certain
newcomers to an organisation may require more support and may therefore benefit
from additional interventions such as autonomy-supportive mentoring (e.g. Janessen
et al., 2013). Finally, although it has been suggested that enduring individual
differences in causality orientation cannot be changed (Gagné & Deci, 2005), it has
been found that ‘cluster membership’ can change in certain domains. For example,
Hayenga & Corpus (2010) found that within an autonomy-supportive academic
context 43% of participants changed cluster membership. Future research to
establish this finding would be valuable. If supported, it would suggest that further
benefits could be obtained from autonomy-support through its ability to encourage
development of self-determined profiles, which, in turn, can help to retain the positive
effects of an initial intervention.
32
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
4.5. CONCLUSION
This study has emphasised the profound influence the perception of autonomy-
support can have upon a student-aged population, through only a short interaction.
These findings are of significance to various life domains in which the promotion of
high-quality motivation and facilitation of performance is invaluable. In particular, this
research demonstrates the utility of an autonomy-supportive motivational style as an
innovative technique to enhance performance-related outcomes in the next
generation of workers. In the face of rising pressure, competition and demand,
modern organisations commonly adopt “quick-fix” motivational strategies that may
enhance business productivity but appear to disregard the true value of a key
competitive advantage; their employees. An autonomy-supportive motivational style
provides a novel strategy that has the potential to facilitate autonomous motivation,
enhance employee performance and drive business success.
33
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
REFERENCES
Arnold, J., Silvester, J., Patterson, F., Robertson, I., Cooper, C., & Burnes, B. (Eds.)
(2005). Approaches to work motivation and job design. In Work Psychology,
Understanding Human Behaviour in the Workplace, (pp. 306-352). (4th ed.)
Harlow, UK: Pearson Education Limited.
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic Need Satisfaction: A
Motivational Basis of Performance and Well-Being in Two Work Settings.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 2045-2068.
Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors' autonomy support and
students' autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-
determination theory perspective. Science Education, 84, 740-756.
Bindl, U. K., & Parker, S. K. (2009). Proactive work behavior: Forward-thinking and
change-oriented action in organizations. In S. Zedeck (Eds.), APA handbook
of industrial and organizational psychology. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
V
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Boiché, J. C. S., Sarrazin, P. G., Grouzet, F. M. E., Pelletier, L. G., & Chanal, J. P.
(2008). Students’ Motivational Profiles and Achievement Outcomes in
Physical Education: A Self-Determination Perspective. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 100, 688–701.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985b). The general causality orientations scale: Self-
determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109-
134.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘‘what’’ and ‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits: Human
needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11,
227-268.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Motivation, Personality, and Development Within
Embedded Social Contexts: An Overview of Self-Determination Theory. In
R.M. Ryan (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation (pp. 85-103).
New York: Oxford University Press.
VI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Earley, P. C., Connolly, T., & Ekegren, G. (1989). Goals, strategy development and
task performance: Some limits on the efficacy of goal-setting. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 74, 24-33.
Elst, T. V., Van den Broeck, A., De Witte, H., & De Cuyper, N. (2012). The mediating
role of frustration of psychological needs in the relationship between job
insecurity and work-related well-being. Work & Stress: An International
Journal of Work, Health & Organisations, 26, 252-271.
Fang, M., & Gerhart, B. (2012). Does pay for performance diminish intrinsic interest?
The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 23, 1176-
1196.
Friedman, R., Deci, E. L., Elliot, A. J., Moller, A. C., & Aarts, H. (2010). Motivational
synchronicity: Priming motivational orientations with observations of others’
behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 34-38.
Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work motivation.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 331-362.
VII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Grant, A. M., Nurmohamed, S., Ashford, S. J., & Dekas, K. (2011). The performance
implications of ambivalent initiative: The interplay of autonomous and
controlled motivations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 116, 241-251.
Guay, F., Ratelle, C. F., & Chanal, J. (2008). Optimal learning in optimal contexts:
The role of self-determination in education. Canadian Psychology, 49,
233–240.
Hardré, P. (2003). Beyond two decades of motivation: a review of the research and
practice in human performance technology. Human Resource Development
Review, 2, 54–81.
Hardré, P. L., & Reeve, J. (2009). Training corporate managers to adopt a more
autonomy-supportive motivating style toward employees: an intervention
study. International Journal of Training and Development, 13, 165-184.
Hayenga, A. O., & Corpus, J. H. (2010). Profiles of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations:
A person-centered approach to motivation and achievement in middle school.
Motivation and Emotion, 34, 371–383.
Jacobs, N., Hagger, M. S., Streukens, S., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Claes, N. (2011).
Testing an integrated model of the theory of planned behaviour and self-
determination theory for different energy balance-related behaviours and
intervention intensities. British Journal of Health Psychology, 16, 113–134.
VIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Janssen, S., van Vuuren, M., & de Jong, M. D. T. (2013). Identifying support
functions in developmental relationships: A self-determination perspective.
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 82, 20–29.
Jang, H., Kim, E. J., & Reeve, J. (2012). Longitudinal Test of Self-Determination
Theory’s Motivation Mediation Model in a Naturally Occurring Classroom
Context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 1175–1188.
Jang, H., Reeve, J., Ryan, R. M., & Kim, A. (2009). Can self-determination theory
explain what underlies the productive, satisfying learning experiences of
collectivistically oriented South Korean adolescents? Journal of Educational
Psychology, 101, 644–661.
Jenkins, G. D., Mitra, A., Gupta, N, & Shaw, J. D. (1998). Are financial incentives
related to performance? A meta-analytic review of empirical research. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 83, 777-787.
Jones, F., & Bright, J. (2001). Stress: myth, theory, and research. Harlow, England:
Pearson.
Jones, F. A., Burke, R. J., Westman, M. (2006). Work life Balance: A Psychological
Perspective. New York: Psychology Press.
Kelly, J. E. (1993). Does job redesign theory explain job re-design outcomes?
Human Relations, 45, 753-774.
Kodz, J., Harper, H., & Dench, S. (2002). Work-life balance: Beyond the rhetoric.
Institute for Employment Studies Report 384, London: IES.
Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (2002). Distinguishing three ways of being internally
motivated: a closer look at introjection, identification, and intrinsic motivation.
In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research
(pp. 101–121). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Kouzes, J. and Posner, B. (2002). The Leadership Challenge. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
IX
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Lam, C. F., & Gurland, S. T. (2008). Self-determined work motivation predicts job
outcomes, but what predicts self-determined work motivation? Journal of
Research in Personality, 42, 1109-1115.
Levesque, C., & Pelletier, L. G. (2003). On the investigation of primed and chronic
autonomous and heteronomous motivational orientations. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1570–1584.
Lewis, J.P. (2005). Project Planning, Scheduling and Control (4th. ed.). New York:
McGraw Hill.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Loughlin, C., & Barling, J. (2001). Young workers’ work values, attitudes and
behaviours. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 74,
543-558.
MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation Analysis. Annual
Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614.
Maltby, J., Day, L., & Macaskill, A. (2010). Further Ideas and Debates in Personality:
Personality and Culture. Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence,
(pp. 230-261). (2nd. ed.). Harlow, UK: Pearson Education Limited.
X
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. V. (1989). Psychometric properties of the
Intrinsic Motivation Inventory in a competitive sport setting: A confirmatory
factor analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 60, 48-58.
Moran, C. M., Diefendorff, J. M., Kim, T., & Liu, Z. (2012). A profile approach to self-
determination theory motivations at work. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81,
354–363.
Moreau, E., & Mageau, G. A. (2012). The importance of perceived autonomy support
for the psychological health and work satisfaction of health professionals: Not
only supervisors count, colleagues too! Motivation and Emotion, 36, 268-
286
Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. (2006). The Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ):
Developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design
and the nature of work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1321–1339.
Ng, J. Y. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M.,
Duda, J. L., & Williams, G. C. (2012). Self-Determination Theory Applied to
Health Contexts: A Meta-Analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7,
325-340.
Nunez, J. L., Leon, J., Grijalvo, F., & Martin-Albo, J. (2012). Measuring autonomy
support in university students: The Spanish version of the Learning Climate
Questionnaire. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15, 1466-1472.
Parker, S. K., & Wall, T. D. (2001). Work design: Learning from the past and
mapping a new terrain. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C.
Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work and organizational
psychology: Vol. 1, Personnel psychology (pp. 90–109). London: Sage.
XI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Pulfrey, C., Darnon, C., & Butera, F. (2013). Autonomy and Task Performance:
Explaining the Impact of Grades on Intrinsic Motivation. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 105, 39–57.
Ratelle, C. F., Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., Larose, S., & Senécal, C. (2007).
Autonomous, controlled, and amotivated types of academic motivation: A
person-oriented analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 734-746.
Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students
and how they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational
Psychologist, 44, 159–75.
Reeve, J. & Jang, H. (2006). What Teachers Say and Do to Support Students’
Autonomy During a Learning Activity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98,
209-218.
Richer, S. F., Blanchard, C., & Vallerand, R. J. (2002). A motivational model of work
turnover. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 2089–2113.
Ryan, R. M., Chandler, C., Connell, J. P., & Deci, E. L. (1985). Internalization and
motivation: Some preliminary research and theoretical speculations. In Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development. Detroit, MI.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization:
Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 57, 749-761.
XII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Plant, R. W. (1990). Emotions in non-directed text
learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 2, 1-17.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of
intrinsic motivation, social development and well-being. American
Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human
autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will?
Journal of Personality, 74, 1557-1585.
Ryan, R. M., Mims, V., and Koestner, R. (1983). Relation of Reward Contingency
and Interpersonal Context to Intrinsic Motivation: A Review and Test Using
Cognitive Evaluation Theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
45, 736–750.
Sheldon, K. M., & Grunz, A. (2009). Psychological needs as basic motives, not just
experiential requirements. Journal of Personality, 77, 1467-92.
Sheldon, K. M., & Niemiec, C. P. (2006). It’s not just the amount that counts:
Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 91, 331–341.
XIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Van den Broeck, A., Lens, W., De Whitte, H., & Van Coillie, H. (2013). Unraveling
the importance of the quantity and the quality of workers’ motivation for well-
being: A person-centered perspective. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 82,
69-78.
Van den Broeck, A., Schreurs, B., De Witte, H., Vansteenkiste, M., Germeys, F., &
Schaufeli, W. (2011). Understanding Workaholics’ Motivations: A Self-
Determination Perspective. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 60,
600-621.
Westover, J. H., & Taylor, J. (2010). International differences in job satisfaction: The
effects of public service motivation, rewards and work relations. International
Journal of Productivity and Performance Management, 59, 811-828.
Zhang, J., Liu, G., Shi, W., & Fu, X. (2011). On the relationship between motivating
style and elementary students’ creative thinking: The mediating role of
autonomous motivation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 43, 1138-1150.
XIV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
APPENDIX
Autonomy-Supportive Controlling
Time Listening *Time Talking
*Seating arrangements
Whether student allowed to sit closet to *Uttering directives/commands
learning materials
*Communicating perspective-taking
statements
where:
XV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
2 A. Intrinsic Motivation
μ1
σ1
Condition N Mean Std. Deviation
Autonomy-Supportive 35 4.0726 .39235
σ2
σ' = √ (.39235 + .55002 ) / 2
2 2 μ2
σ' = 0.4777…
σ' = 0.478 (to 3 s.f.)
To calculate Cohen’s d substitute the pooled standard deviation (σ') into the equation:
2B. Performance
μ1 σ1
Condition N Mean Std. Deviation
Autonomy-Supportive 35 83.71 11.398
σ2
μ2
σ' = √ (11.3982 + 16.3212) / 2
σ' = 14.0763…
σ' = 14.1 (to 3 s.f.)
To calculate Cohen’s d substitute the pooled standard deviation (σ') into the equation:
XVI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
N
d
2
3 A. Intrinsic Motivation
δ = 1.68 √ 35/2
δ = 7.0279…
δ = 7.03 (to 3 s.f.)
3B. Performance
δ = 0.56 √ 35/2
δ = 2.3426…
δ = 2.34 (to 3 s.f.)
XVII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Task 1:
“The first task is a reading task. This task is looking into the way that you learn and the
techniques that you use.” Providing a Rationale
“Which passage would you like?” *Holding out the two copies of the Reading Task* Providing Choice
It is a passage about computer use in young children. In a moment I will leave the room and
let you read through the passage as many times as you want. I will come back in after 10
minutes but please don’t feel you have to use all this time if you want to stop reading sooner
just call me back in – it is totally up to you!
Providing Choice – How to complete the task and Timeframe needed
I have also left some paper and a pen here – you could use this to make notes/draw
pictures/spider diagrams if you wish. You decide how you would like to tackle this task, use
whatever technique you think works best for you. Providing Choice – How to
complete the task
You can do it! Providing Encouragement
Do you have any questions? Providing time for Questions
“I’ll let you know when we will start the next task.” **Time for 4 minutes**
“Right… Are you ready for the next task?” Hinting the Participant has control
XVIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Task 2:
“They are all multiple choice questions and I am sure you will be absolutely fine answering
them!” Providing Encouragement
“You have 8 minutes to answer the questions, but again if you want to finish before this time
let me know and you can stop the task. You can just go straight in and start the questions,
read them through before answering, leave questions out and return to them. Use whatever
approach you feel will work best for you.” There is some more paper you could use if you
wish” Providing Choice
“Do you have any questions?”
Providing the opportunity for questions
*Time for 8 Minutes*
Task 3:
“Finally, if you don’t mind the last task is a questionnaire. It isn’t too long and you can take as
long as you like to complete it.” Providing Choice
“I know you have been here a little while but please could you read through the questions
carefully and think through your answers. This task simply aims to understand your
experience of the Study, there are no trick questions just say how you feel.”
Providing a Rationale
“Remember this is completely anonymous – and all opinions/information you give is
completely confidential. Once you have completed your questionnaire please put it into the
envelope provided so I will not see your answers.”
Attempt to prevent social desirability
“That is the end of the Study, thank you very much for your participation”
*Provide Debrief*
XIX
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
You must read through this information sheet carefully and complete this consent form giving
your informed consent to complete this Study. Read through the questions on the consent
form carefully and mark your answer on the side.
Task 1:
*Still holding the materials*
“In a moment I will leave the room and you must read through a passage. I will return in 10
minutes to stop you reading. By this time you should have finished reading.”
Given a strict deadline
No opportunity for
*Give them the ‘controlling’ reading task, leave the room and time 10 minutes* questions given
Indicates the Instructor is in
Time up! *Instructor takes the papers away* control
“Now you must sit quietly for the next few minutes.” No rationale given
*Time for 4 minutes*
“The next task you have to complete is a comprehension task.” No rationale given
“It is composed of 10 questions. You have 8 minutes to answer the questions, by this time
you should have answered all questions.” Given a strict deadline
No opportunity for questions given
*Stop task after 8 minutes, Instructor takes away the papers*
Indicates the Instructor is in control
Task 3:
“The last task is a questionnaire. You can have up to 15 minutes to complete it.”
No rationale provided Given a strict deadline
“You should read through the questions carefully and you must be honest with your answers.
The Questionnaire is completely anonymous and all opinions/information you give is
completely confidential. To ensure this you have to the completed questionnaire into the
envelope provided so I will not be able to see your answers.” No opportunity for questions given
“That is the end of the Study, thank you very much for your participation”
*Provide Debrief*
Note. Circled words throughout the instructions indicate controlling commands or directives
used
XX
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
The role of computers in the development of a young child has been a widely
controversial topic for decades, and both parents and educators have put forth both
concerns about the potential benefits as well as harms to young children. Critics argue
that introducing technology in schools only wastes money and time, and that instead
children should be allowed to develop essential learning and social skills through
interaction with other students. On the other hand, proponents to the idea suggest that
children should take advantage of the newest technologies and that children should
learn to become adept at utilizing such technologies as a means to further their success
in their eventual entering of the workforce. There are also some concerns that the most
modern technologies are not being optimized and utilized in the best way possible.
Both critics and proponents of computers in the classroom agree that the early,
formative years of any child are when physical, social-emotional, language, and
cognitive skills are acquired. Perhaps the most researched area of development in
relation to computer use has been that of cognitive development and the affect that
modern technology has on a child’s mind. Are computers being used properly to
enhance and hasten a child’s cognitive development, or are they inhibiting intellectual
growth? Can technology support the specific needs of children, or does it take away
from essential developmental experiences?
XXI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
The role of computers in the development of a young child has been a widely controversial topic
for decades, and both parents and educators have put forth both concerns about the potential
benefits as well as harms to young children. Critics argue that introducing technology in schools
only wastes money and time, and that instead children should be allowed to develop essential
learning and social skills through interaction with other students. On the other hand, proponents
to the idea suggest that children should take advantage of the newest technologies and that
children should learn to become adept at utilizing such technologies as a means to further their
success in their eventual entering of the workforce. There are also some concerns that the most
modern technologies are not being optimized and utilized in the best way possible.
Both critics and proponents of computers in the classroom agree that the early, formative years of
any child are when physical, social-emotional, language, and cognitive skills are acquired.
Perhaps the most researched area of development in relation to computer use has been that of
cognitive development and the affect that modern technology has on a child’s mind. Are
computers being used properly to enhance and hasten a child’s cognitive development, or are they
inhibiting intellectual growth? Can technology support the specific needs of children, or does it
take away from essential developmental experiences?
Recent research on brain development has focused on the capabilities of young children, the
stages and styles of learning, and social-emotional development. Such research has shown that
although children may lack knowledge and experience, they have ample reasoning ability. Given
appropriate stimuli, such as close interaction with caring adults and engaging hands-on activities,
most children have been shown to dramatically improve their mental developmental skills. A
study by the National Research Council found that early learning is assisted by the supportive
context of the family and the social environment, through the kinds of activities in which adults
engage with children. The influence of the two most renowned learning theories of psychology,
Piaget’s theory and Vygotsky’s constructivism theory, are evident in the most recent research
efforts, and it is in considering their models of development that we can make some assessment
about the significance of a computer’s role in a child's development process. Researchers have
attempted to apply the developmental theories of Piaget to children’s computer usage. In
considering the Piagetian tasks of classifying and categorization, researchers have made several
interesting observations about computers and cognitive development. For example, it has been
suggested that a child sorting grocery items in the kitchen is a sign of mental development.
XXII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Task 2: Comprehension
Please could you now complete the following 10 questions regarding the passage you have just
read.
This exercise is looking into how differing learning styles affect an individual’s memory. You will Providing
Rationale
have 8 minutes to complete this task, but feel free to let me know if you want to finish early.
Just try your best and I am sure you will do well! Providing Choice
Providing Encouragement
1. Which of the following best describes the development and organization of the
passage?
a. The author begins with a concise introduction, followed with a thorough analysis of the
shortcomings of using computers in the classroom.
b. After a broad overview of the argument, the author discusses recent trends in research,
followed by a short description of how proponents and advocates of technology in the
classroom agree on several key issues.
c. First the two viewpoints are introduced, followed by an analysis of the similarities of the
arguments for and against using technology in the classroom, and finally current research
trends are briefly discussed.
d. The author first selects to advocate the use of computer technology in the classroom, but
then, upon a closer inspection of the arguments and research trends, ultimately decides to
shun the use of modern technologies in education.
XXIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
6. A study by the National Research Council found early learning is assisted by:
a. Supportive families and social environment
b. Use of Computers
c. Healthy lifestyle
d. Both a and b
7. Piaget and Vygotsky provide two separate theories, but both incorporate a
model of:
a. Learning
b. Development
c. Cognition
d. None of the above
9. The text mentions a study in the last sentence that suggests child sorting
grocery items in the kitchen is a sign of:
a. Mental Development
b. Intellect
c. Social Skills
d. Mathematical Skills
10. The passage provides information that could be used to answer which of the
following questions?
a. Approximately during which years of a child's life are physical, social, emotional,
communication and cognitive skills acquired?
b. Does a young child sorting grocery items provide proof that social and communication skills
are lacking?
c. Should computer technologies be introduced to students when they are in their teens?
d. Has research shown that the use of computers helps to enhance a child's cognitive
development?
That is the end of the Comprehension Task. Thank you for participating.
XXIV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Task 2: Comprehension
You now have to complete the following 10 questions. You must answer every question. You have 8
minutes to complete the task, by which time you should have answered all the questions.
1. Which of the following best describes the development and organization of the passage?
a. The author begins with a concise introduction, followed with a thorough analysis of the
shortcomings of using computers in the classroom.
b. After a broad overview of the argument, the author discusses recent trends in research,
followed by a short description of how proponents and advocates of technology in the
classroom agree on several key issues.
c. First the two viewpoints are introduced, followed by an analysis of the similarities of the
arguments for and against using technology in the classroom, and finally current
research trends are briefly discussed.
d. The author first selects to advocate the use of computer technology in the classroom, but
then, upon a closer inspection of the arguments and research trends, ultimately decides
to shun the use of modern technologies in education.
3. “…early, formative years of any child are when physical, social-emotional, language
and cognitive skills are acquired.” Who agrees with this statement?
a. Critics of child computer use
b. Parents
c. Proponents of child computer use
d. Both a and c
XXV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
6. A study by the National Research Council found early learning is assisted by:
a. Supportive families and social environment
b. Use of Computers
c. Healthy lifestyle
d. Both a and b
7. Piaget and Vygotsky provide two separate theories, but both incorporate a model of:
a. Learning
b. Development
c. Cognition
d. None of the above
9. The text mentions a study in the last sentence that suggests child sorting grocery
items in the kitchen is a sign of:
a. Mental Development
b. Intellect
c. Social Skills
d. Mathematical Skills
10. The passage provides information that could be used to answer which of the following
questions?
a. Approximately during which years of a child's life are physical, social, emotional,
communication and cognitive skills acquired?
b. Does a young child sorting grocery items provide proof that social and communication
skills are lacking?
c. Should computer technologies be introduced to students when they are in their teens?
d. Has research shown that the use of computers helps to enhance a child's cognitive
development?
That is the end of the Comprehension Task. Thank you for participating.
Note. Correct answers are equivalent to those in previous comprehension task (Appendix 9.)
XXVI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
____ This activity did not hold my attention at all. EN2 (Reversed Question)
____ I think I did pretty well at this activity, compared to other students. CO2
____ I believe I had some choice about doing this activity. CH3
____ I did not feel nervous at all while doing this. T2 (Reversed Question)
____ After working at this activity for a while, I felt pretty competent. CO3
____ I did not put much energy into this. EF3 (Reversed Question)
____ I did not try very hard to do well at this activity. EF4 (Reversed Question)
____ This was an activity that I could not do very well. CO5 (Reversed Question)
____ I felt like it was not my own choice to do this task. CH4 (Reversed Question)
XXVII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
I do not feel very good about the way my instructor talked to me. PAS4
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
disagree agree
My instructor made sure I really understood the task and what I needed to do. PAS5
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
disagree agree
My instructor did not give me freedom and choice in the task. PAS6
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
disagree agree
_________________________________________________________________________
Thank you for your time. That is the end of the Questionnaire. Please put the
Questionnaire in the Envelope provided and then call in the Instructor.
Measure taken to Prevent Social Desirability
XXVIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Information Sheet
E-Mail: ps10ss@leeds.ac.uk
This research is subject to ethical guidelines set out by the British Psychological
Society. These guidelines include principles such as obtaining your informed consent
before research starts, notifying you of your right to withdraw, and protection of your
anonymity. This sheet has been designed to provide you with enough information
about the study to allow you to make an informed decision about participation.
Please take time to read the following information carefully and if you have any
questions or would like to discuss anything with me please let me know.
Study Procedure
The Study will consist of three tasks. The first task will involve reading a passage of
information on computer use in young children. The second task requires you to
complete a short comprehension exercise. Finally, you will be provided with a short
questionnaire to complete.
The Study is expected to last for approximately 30-45 minutes and you will receive 2
Participant Pool Credits for you participation.
Participation in this study is voluntary. If you agree to participate in this study, all
your responses and your questionnaire will be treated confidentially. Your name and
identifying information (provided on your consent form) will be kept securely
and separately from the rest of your experimental materials, including your
questionnaire. If you decide to participate, you are free to withdraw at any time and
without giving a reason.
XXIX
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Every Participant was required to complete this form in order to participate in the Study
Consent Form
Signed:
__________________________________________________
Date:
__________________________________________________
XXX
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
The Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000) proposes
that individuals have innate psychological needs for competence, autonomy and
relatedness. Based on the postulates of SDT, research has been conducted to assess
the benefits of autonomy-support, specifically the benefits of providing autonomy-
supportive teaching and coaching. It has been shown that this supportive style increases
motivation and engagement, resulting in better performance outcomes.
There were two conditions in this Study, one in which the instructor was autonomy-
supportive and displayed certain behaviours to support the Participant throughout the
task, including providing rationales, choice and encouragement. The second condition is
a ‘controlling’ condition in which the Participants were not supported through the task
and were simply told what to do and how to do it. It is hypothesised that compared to
participants that were in the controlling condition, participants in the autonomy-
supportive condition would be more motivated to complete the comprehension task. As
a result, it is expected that Participants in the autonomy-supportive condition will
demonstrate better task performance. The Questionnaire assessed both level of intrinsic
motivation (page 1.) and the perception of autonomy-support provided by the instructor
(page 2.), as a manipulation check. Performance was assessed through the Multiple-
Choice Test.
This research is important because it will help psychologists to understand the best
techniques to motivate performance in Students. It also has wider implications in the
workplace, specifically in terms of young workers entering the workforce and how
Management can support their needs and motivation. As a potential strategy for
enhancing employee motivation and performance generally, this technique would be of
great interest to employers dealing with issues, such as employee engagement, as a
result of the current economic situation. Finally, this research may provide Employers
with a tool to improve both organisational performance and employee wellbeing.
If you would like more information about this research, please contact:
XXXI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Descriptives
Descriptive Statistics
N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation
Age 70 18 25 19.31 1.136
Valid N (listwise) 70
Descriptive Statistics
Condition N Mean Std. Deviation
Autonomy_Support 35 6.6286 .38579
Intrinsic_Motivation 35 4.0726 .39235
Enjoy 35 4.1571 .60085
Choice 35 5.0214 .62536
Autonomy-Supportive Effort 35 4.5143 .84875
Tension 35 2.2857 .88106
Competence 35 4.2524 .55479
Performance 35 83.71 11.398
Valid N (listwise) 35
Autonomy_Support 35 2.9190 1.29691
Intrinsic_Motivation 35 3.2690 .55002
Enjoy 35 2.4619 .97860
Choice 35 3.6714 1.13561
Controlling Effort 35 3.7857 1.05570
Tension 35 2.7929 1.32909
Competence 35 3.7810 .76672
Performance 35 75.71 16.321
Valid N (listwise) 35
XXXII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Oneway
ANOVA
Intrinsic_Motivation
Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Between Groups 11.300 1 11.300 49.512 .000
Within Groups 15.520 68 .228
Total 26.820 69
ANOVA
Performance
Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Between Groups 1120.000 1 1120.000 5.652 .020
Within Groups 13474.286 68 198.151
Total 14594.286 69
XXXIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Correlations
Correlations
Intrinsic_Motivation Autonomy_Support
Pearson Correlation 1 .665**
Intrinsic_Motivation Sig. (2-tailed) .000
N 70 70
**
Pearson Correlation .665 1
Autonomy_Support Sig. (2-tailed) .000
N 70 70
Correlations
Performance Autonomy_Support
Pearson Correlation 1 .190
Performance Sig. (2-tailed) .116
N 70 70
Pearson Correlation .190 1
Autonomy_Support Sig. (2-tailed) .116
N 70 70
Correlations
Performance Intrinsic_Motivation
Pearson Correlation 1 .333**
Performance Sig. (2-tailed) .005
N 70 70
Pearson Correlation .333** 1
Intrinsic_Motivation Sig. (2-tailed) .005
N 70 70
XXXIV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Regression 1
Variables Entered/Removeda
Model Variables Variables Method
Entered Removed
Autonomy_Sup
1 . Enter
portb
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
b. All requested variables entered.
Model Summary
Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the
Square Estimate
a
1 .665 .443 .434 .46885
a. Predictors: (Constant), Autonomy_Support
ANOVAa
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 11.872 1 11.872 54.008 .000b
1 Residual 14.948 68 .220
Total 26.820 69
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
b. Predictors: (Constant), Autonomy_Support
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 2.726 .140 19.436 .000
1
Autonomy_Support .198 .027 .665 7.349 .000
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
Regression 2
Variables Entered/Removeda
Model Variables Variables Method
Entered Removed
Intrinsic_Motiva
1 . Enter
tionb
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
Model Summary
Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the
Square Estimate
a
1 .333 .111 .098 13.813
a. Predictors: (Constant), Intrinsic_Motivation
XXXV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
ANOVAa
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 1619.596 1 1619.596 8.488 .005b
1 Residual 12974.690 68 190.804
Total 14594.286 69
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
b. Predictors: (Constant), Intrinsic_Motivation
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 51.188 9.929 5.155 .000
1
Intrinsic_Motivation 7.771 2.667 .333 2.913 .005
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
XXXVI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Model Summary
Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the
Square Estimate
a
1 .665 .443 .434 .46885
a. Predictors: (Constant), Autonomy_Support
ANOVAa
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 11.872 1 11.872 54.008 .000b
1 Residual 14.948 68 .220
Total 26.820 69
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
b. Predictors: (Constant), Autonomy_Support
XXXVII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 2.726 .140 19.436 .000
1
Autonomy_Support .198 .027 .665 7.349 .000
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
XXXVIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Model Summary
Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the
Square Estimate
a
1 .277 .077 .063 14.077
a. Predictors: (Constant), Condition
ANOVAa
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 1120.000 1 1120.000 5.652 .020b
1 Residual 13474.286 68 198.151
Total 14594.286 69
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
b. Predictors: (Constant), Condition
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 91.714 5.320 17.238 .000
1
Condition -8.000 3.365 -.277 -2.377 .020
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
XXXIX
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
XL
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 61.578 17.945 3.431 .001
1 Intrinsic_Motivation 6.180 3.520 .265 1.756 .084
Condition -3.034 4.357 -.105 -.696 .489
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
XLI
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Model Summary
Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the
Square Estimate
a
1 .665 .443 .434 .46885
a. Predictors: (Constant), Autonomy_Support
ANOVAa
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 11.872 1 11.872 54.008 .000b
1 Residual 14.948 68 .220
Total 26.820 69
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
b. Predictors: (Constant), Autonomy_Support
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 2.726 .140 19.436 .000
1
Autonomy_Support .198 .027 .665 7.349 .000
a. Dependent Variable: Intrinsic_Motivation
Model Summary
Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the
Square Estimate
a
1 .333 .111 .098 13.813
a. Predictors: (Constant), Intrinsic_Motivation
ANOVAa
Model Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.
Regression 1619.596 1 1619.596 8.488 .005b
1 Residual 12974.690 68 190.804
Total 14594.286 69
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
b. Predictors: (Constant), Intrinsic_Motivation
Coefficientsa
Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 51.188 9.929 5.155 .000
1
Intrinsic_Motivation 7.771 2.667 .333 2.913 .005
a. Dependent Variable: Performance
XLII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
XLIII
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
XLIV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
Student’s Signature:
Supervisor’s Signature:
XLV
MOTIVATION & AUTONOMY -SUPPORT
XLVI