Color
Color
PDF generated using the open source mwlib toolkit. See http://code.pediapress.com/ for more information.
PDF generated at: Mon, 28 Jan 2013 13:15:29 UTC
Contents
Articles
Color 1
Color Theory 12
Color space 12
Color theory 16
Additive color 23
Subtractive color 25
Mixing Color 28
Color mixing 28
Primary color 29
Colorfulness 35
Dichromatism 39
Hue 41
Tints and shades 44
Lightness 46
Perception of Color 50
Opponent process 50
Impossible colors 53
Color vision 54
Visual perception 64
Visual Color 70
List of colors 70
Web colors 89
References
Article Sources and Contributors 100
Image Sources, Licenses and Contributors 103
Article Licenses
License 105
Color 1
Color
Color or colour (see spelling differences) is
the visual perceptual property corresponding
in humans to the categories called red, blue,
yellow, green and others. Color derives from
the spectrum of light (distribution of light
power versus wavelength) interacting in the
eye with the spectral sensitivities of the light
receptors. Color categories and physical
specifications of color are also associated
with objects, materials, light sources, etc.,
based on their physical properties such as
light absorption, reflection, or emission
spectra. By defining a color space, colors
Colored pencils
can be identified numerically by their
coordinates.
Because perception of color stems from the varying spectral sensitivity of different types of cone cells in the retina to
different parts of the spectrum, colors may be defined and quantified by the degree to which they stimulate these
cells. These physical or physiological quantifications of color, however, do not fully explain the psychophysical
perception of color appearance.
The science of color is sometimes called chromatics, chromatography, colorimetry, or simply color science. It
includes the perception of color by the human eye and brain, the origin of color in materials, color theory in art, and
the physics of electromagnetic radiation in the visible range (that is, what we commonly refer to simply as light).
Physics
Electromagnetic radiation is characterized by its wavelength (or frequency) and its intensity. When the wavelength is
within the visible spectrum (the range of wavelengths humans can perceive, approximately from 390 nm to 700 nm),
it is known as "visible light".
Most light sources emit light at many different wavelengths; a source's spectrum is a distribution giving its intensity
at each wavelength. Although the spectrum of light arriving at the eye from a given direction determines the color
sensation in that direction, there are many more possible spectral combinations than color sensations. In fact, one
may formally define a color as a class of spectra that give rise to the same color sensation, although such classes
would vary widely among different species, and to a lesser extent among individuals within the same species. In
each such class the members are called metamers of the color in question.
Spectral colors
The familiar colors of the rainbow in the spectrum – named using the Latin word for appearance or apparition by
Isaac Newton in 1671 – include all those colors that can be produced by visible light of a single wavelength only, the
pure spectral or monochromatic colors. The table at right shows approximate frequencies (in terahertz) and
wavelengths (in nanometers) for various pure spectral colors. The wavelengths are measured in air or vacuum (see
refraction).
The color table should not be interpreted as a definitive list – the pure spectral colors form a continuous spectrum,
and how it is divided into distinct colors linguistically is a matter of culture and historical contingency (although
people everywhere have been shown to perceive colors in the same way[2]). A common list identifies six main
bands: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet. Newton's conception included a seventh color, indigo, between
blue and violet. Optical scientists Hardy and Perrin list indigo as between 446 and 464 nm wavelength.[3]
The intensity of a spectral color, relative to the context in which it is viewed, may alter its perception considerably;
for example, a low-intensity orange-yellow is brown, and a low-intensity yellow-green is olive-green.
For discussion of non-spectral colors, see below.
Color of objects
The color of an object depends on both the physics of the object in its environment and the characteristics of the
perceiving eye and brain. Physically, objects can be said to have the color of the light leaving their surfaces, which
normally depends on the spectrum of the incident illumination and the reflectance properties of the surface, as well
as potentially on the angles of illumination and viewing. Some objects not only reflect light, but also transmit light or
emit light themselves (see below), which contribute to the color also. And a viewer's perception of the object's color
Color 3
depends not only on the spectrum of the light leaving its surface, but also on a host of contextual cues, so that the
color tends to be perceived as relatively constant: that is, relatively independent of the lighting spectrum, viewing
angle, etc. This effect is known as color constancy.
Some generalizations of the physics can be drawn,
neglecting perceptual effects for now:
• Light arriving at an opaque surface is either
reflected "specularly" (that is, in the manner of a
mirror), scattered (that is, reflected with diffuse
scattering), or absorbed – or some combination of
these.
• Opaque objects that do not reflect specularly (which
tend to have rough surfaces) have their color
determined by which wavelengths of light they
scatter more and which they scatter less (with the
light that is not scattered being absorbed). If objects
scatter all wavelengths, they appear white. If they The upper disk and the lower disk have exactly the same objective
color, and are in identical gray surroundings; based on context
absorb all wavelengths, they appear black.
differences, humans perceive the squares as having different
• Opaque objects that specularly reflect light of reflectances, and may interpret the colors as different color
different wavelengths with different efficiencies categories; see checker shadow illusion.
look like mirrors tinted with colors determined by
those differences. An object that reflects some fraction of impinging light and absorbs the rest may look black but
also be faintly reflective; examples are black objects coated with layers of enamel or lacquer.
• Objects that transmit light are either translucent (scattering the transmitted light) or transparent (not scattering the
transmitted light). If they also absorb (or reflect) light of various wavelengths differentially, they appear tinted
with a color determined by the nature of that absorption (or that reflectance).
• Objects may emit light that they generate themselves, rather than merely reflecting or transmitting light. They
may do so because of their elevated temperature (they are then said to be incandescent), as a result of certain
chemical reactions (a phenomenon called chemoluminescence), or for other reasons (see the articles
Phosphorescence and List of light sources).
• Objects may absorb light and then as a consequence emit light that has different properties. They are then called
fluorescent (if light is emitted only while light is absorbed) or phosphorescent (if light is emitted even after light
ceases to be absorbed; this term is also sometimes loosely applied to light emitted because of chemical reactions).
For further treatment of the color of objects, see structural color, below.
To summarize, the color of an object is a complex result of its surface properties, its transmission properties, and its
emission properties, all of which factors contribute to the mix of wavelengths in the light leaving the surface of the
object. The perceived color is then further conditioned by the nature of the ambient illumination, and by the color
properties of other objects nearby, via the effect known as color constancy and via other characteristics of the
perceiving eye and brain.
Color 4
Perception
At the same time as Helmholtz, Ewald Hering developed the opponent process theory of color, noting that color
blindness and afterimages typically come in opponent pairs (red-green, blue-orange, yellow-purple, and
black-white). Ultimately these two theories were synthesized in 1957 by Hurvich and Jameson, who showed that
retinal processing corresponds to the trichromatic theory, while processing at the level of the lateral geniculate
nucleus corresponds to the opponent theory.[5]
In 1931, an international group of experts known as the Commission internationale de l'éclairage (CIE) developed a
mathematical color model, which mapped out the space of observable colors and assigned a set of three numbers to
each.
Color 5
The exact nature of color perception beyond the processing already described, and indeed the status of color as a
feature of the perceived world or rather as a feature of our perception of the world, is a matter of complex and
continuing philosophical dispute (see qualia).
Color deficiency
If one or more types of a person's color-sensing cones are missing or less responsive than normal to incoming light,
that person can distinguish fewer colors and is said to be color deficient or color blind (though this latter term can be
misleading; almost all color deficient individuals can distinguish at least some colors). Some kinds of color
deficiency are caused by anomalies in the number or nature of cones in the retina. Others (like central or cortical
achromatopsia) are caused by neural anomalies in those parts of the brain where visual processing takes place.
Tetrachromacy
While most humans are trichromatic (having three types of color receptors), many animals, known as tetrachromats,
have four types. These include some species of spiders, most marsupials, birds, reptiles, and many species of fish.
Other species are sensitive to only two axes of color or do not perceive color at all; these are called dichromats and
monochromats respectively. A distinction is made between retinal tetrachromacy (having four pigments in cone
cells in the retina, compared to three in trichromats) and functional tetrachromacy (having the ability to make
enhanced color discriminations based on that retinal difference). As many as half of all women are retinal
tetrachromats.[8]:p.256 The phenomenon arises when an individual receives two slightly different copies of the gene
for either the medium- or long-wavelength cones, which are carried on the x-chromosome. To have two different
genes, a person must have two x-chromosomes, which is why the phenomenon only occurs in women.[8] For some of
these retinal tetrachromats, color discriminations are enhanced, making them functional tetrachromats.[8]
Color 7
Synesthesia
In certain forms of synesthesia, perceiving letters and numbers (grapheme–color synesthesia) or hearing musical
sounds (music–color synesthesia) will lead to the unusual additional experiences of seeing colors. Behavioral and
functional neuroimaging experiments have demonstrated that these color experiences lead to changes in behavioral
tasks and lead to increased activation of brain regions involved in color perception, thus demonstrating their reality,
and similarity to real color percepts, albeit evoked through a non-standard route.
Afterimages
After exposure to strong light in their sensitivity range,
photoreceptors of a given type become desensitized. For a few
seconds after the light ceases, they will continue to signal less
strongly than they otherwise would. Colors observed during
that period will appear to lack the color component detected
by the desensitized photoreceptors. This effect is responsible
for the phenomenon of afterimages, in which the eye may
continue to see a bright figure after looking away from it, but
in a complementary color.
Color naming
Colors vary in several different ways, including hue (shades of red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet),
saturation, brightness, and gloss. Some color words are derived from the name of an object of that color, such as
"orange" or "salmon", while others are abstract, like "red".
In the 1969 study Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution, Brent Berlin and Paul Kay describe a
pattern in naming "basic" colors (like "red" but not "red-orange" or "dark red" or "blood red", which are "shades" of
red). All languages that have two "basic" color names distinguish dark/cool colors from bright/warm colors. The next
colors to be distinguished are usually red and then yellow or green. All languages with six "basic" colors include
black, white, red, green, blue, and yellow. The pattern holds up to a set of twelve: black, gray, white, pink, red,
Color 8
orange, yellow, green, blue, purple, brown, and azure (distinct from blue in Russian and Italian, but not English).
Associations
Individual colors have a variety of cultural associations such as national colors (in general described in individual
color articles and color symbolism). The field of color psychology attempts to identify the effects of color on human
emotion and activity. Chromotherapy is a form of alternative medicine attributed to various Eastern traditions.
Colors have different associations in different countries and cultures.[11]
Different colors have been demonstrated to have effects on cognition. For example, researchers at the University of
Linz in Austria demonstrated that the color red significantly decreases cognitive functioning in men.[12]
Similarly, most human color perceptions can be generated by a mixture of three colors called primaries. This is used
to reproduce color scenes in photography, printing, television, and other media. There are a number of methods or
color spaces for specifying a color in terms of three particular primary colors. Each method has its advantages and
disadvantages depending on the particular application.
No mixture of colors, however, can produce a fully pure color perceived as completely identical to a spectral color,
although one can get very close for the longer wavelengths, where the chromaticity diagram above has a nearly
straight edge. For example, mixing green light (530 nm) and blue light (460 nm) produces cyan light that is slightly
desaturated, because response of the red color receptor would be greater to the green and blue light in the mixture
than it would be to a pure cyan light at 485 nm that has the same intensity as the mixture of blue and green.
Color 9
Because of this, and because the primaries in color printing systems generally are not pure themselves, the colors
reproduced are never perfectly saturated colors, and so spectral colors cannot be matched exactly. However, natural
scenes rarely contain fully saturated colors, thus such scenes can usually be approximated well by these systems. The
range of colors that can be reproduced with a given color reproduction system is called the gamut. The CIE
chromaticity diagram can be used to describe the gamut.
Another problem with color reproduction systems is connected with the acquisition devices, like cameras or
scanners. The characteristics of the color sensors in the devices are often very far from the characteristics of the
receptors in the human eye. In effect, acquisition of colors that have some special, often very "jagged", spectra
caused for example by unusual lighting of the photographed scene can be relatively poor.
Species that have color receptors different from humans – such as bird species, which may have four receptors – can
make color discriminations that humans cannot. A color reproduction system "tuned" to a human with normal color
vision may give very inaccurate results for the other observers, human or non-human.
The different color response of different devices can be problematic if not properly managed. For color information
stored and transferred in digital form, color management techniques, such as those based on ICC profiles, can help to
avoid distortions of the reproduced colors. Color management does not circumvent the gamut limitations of
particular output devices, but can assist in finding good mapping of input colors into the gamut that can be
reproduced.
Structural color
Structural colors are colors caused by interference effects rather than by pigments. Color effects are produced when a
material is scored with fine parallel lines, formed of one or more parallel thin layers, or otherwise composed of
microstructures on the scale of the color's wavelength. If the microstructures are spaced randomly, light of shorter
wavelengths will be scattered preferentially to produce Tyndall effect colors: the blue of the sky (Rayleigh
scattering, caused by structures much smaller than the wavelength of light, in this case air molecules), the luster of
opals, and the blue of human irises. If the microstructures are aligned in arrays, for example the array of pits in a CD,
they behave as a diffraction grating: the grating reflects different wavelengths in different directions due to
interference phenomena, separating mixed "white" light into light of different wavelengths. If the structure is one or
more thin layers then it will reflect some wavelengths and transmit others, depending on the layers' thickness.
Structural color is studied in the field of thin-film optics. A layman's term that describes particularly the most
ordered or the most changeable structural colors is iridescence. Structural color is responsible for the blues and
greens of the feathers of many birds (the blue jay, for example), as well as certain butterfly wings and beetle shells.
Variations in the pattern's spacing often give rise to an iridescent effect, as seen in peacock feathers, soap bubbles,
films of oil, and mother of pearl, because the reflected color depends upon the viewing angle. Numerous scientists
have carried out research in butterfly wings and beetle shells, including Isaac Newton and Robert Hooke. Since
Color 10
1942, electron micrography has been used, advancing the development of products that exploit structural color, such
as "photonic" cosmetics.[13]
Additional terms
• Colorfulness, chroma, purity, or saturation: how "intense" or "concentrated" a color is. Technical definitions
distinguish between colorfulness, chroma, and saturation as distinct perceptual attributes and include purity as a
physical quantity. These terms, and others related to light and color are internationally agreed upon and published
in the CIE Lighting Vocabulary.[14] More readily available texts on colorimetry also define and explain these
terms.[10][15]
• Dichromatism: a phenomenon where the hue is dependent on concentration and/or thickness of the absorbing
substance.
• Hue: the color's direction from white, for example in a color wheel or chromaticity diagram.
• Shade: a color made darker by adding black.
• Tint: a color made lighter by adding white.
• Value, brightness, lightness, or luminosity: how light or dark a color is.
References
[1] Craig F. Bohren (2006). Fundamentals of Atmospheric Radiation: An Introduction with 400 Problems (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=1oDOWr_yueIC& pg=PA214& lpg=PA214& dq=indigo+ spectra+ blue+ violet+ date:1990-2007). Wiley-VCH. ISBN 3-527-40503-8. .
[2] Berlin, B. and Kay, P., Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution, Berkeley: University of California Press, 1969.
[3] Arthur C. Hardy and Fred H. Perrin. The Principles of Optics. (http:/ / apps. isiknowledge. com/ full_record. do?product=UA&
search_mode=GeneralSearch& qid=22& SID=2EdCK2KejLbni4FJpgB& page=1& doc=1& colname=BIOSIS) McGraw-Hill Book Co., Inc.,
New York. 1932.
[4] Hermann von Helmholtz, Physiological Optics – The Sensations of Vision, 1866, as translated in Sources of Color Science, David L.
MacAdam, ed., Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970.
[5] Palmer, S.E. (1999). Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-16183-4.
[6] Judd, Deane B.; Wyszecki, Günter (1975). Color in Business, Science and Industry. Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics (third ed.). New
York: Wiley-Interscience. p. 388. ISBN 0-471-45212-2.
[7] "Under well-lit viewing conditions (photopic vision), cones ...are highly active and rods are inactive." Hirakawa, K.; Parks, T.W. (2005).
"Chromatic Adaptation and White-Balance Problem" (http:/ / www. accidentalmark. com/ research/ papers/ Hirakawa05WBICIP. pdf). IEEE
ICIP. doi:10.1109/ICIP.2005.1530559. .
[8] Jameson, K. A., Highnote, S. M., & Wasserman, L. M. (2001). "Richer color experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin
genes." (http:/ / www. klab. caltech. edu/ cns186/ papers/ Jameson01. pdf) (PDF). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8 (2): 244–261.
doi:10.3758/BF03196159. PMID 11495112. .
[9] Depauw, Robert C.. "United States Patent" (http:/ / www. google. com/ patents?hl=en& lr=& vid=USPAT3815265& id=tSEzAAAAEBAJ&
oi=fnd& dq=mixing+ paint+ colors& printsec=abstract#v=onepage& q=mixing paint colors& f=false). . Retrieved 20 March 2011.
[10] M.D. Fairchild, Color Appearance Models (http:/ / www. wiley. com/ WileyCDA/ WileyTitle/ productCd-0470012161. html), 2nd Ed.,
Wiley, Chichester (2005).
[11] "Chart: Color Meanings by Culture" (http:/ / www. globalization-group. com/ edge/ resources/ color-meanings-by-culture/ ). . Retrieved
2010-06-29.
[12] Gnambs, Timo; Appel, Markus; Batinic, Bernad. (2010). Color red in web-based knowledge testing. Computers in Human Behavior, 26,
p1625-1631.
[13] "Economic and Social Research Council - Science in the Dock, Art in the Stocks" (http:/ / www. esrc. ac. uk/ ESRCInfoCentre/ about/ CI/
events/ FSS/ 2006/ science. aspx?ComponentId=14867& SourcePageId=14865). . Retrieved 2007-10-07.
[14] CIE Pub. 17-4, International Lighting Vocabulary (http:/ / www. cie. co. at/ publ/ abst/ 17-4-89. html), 1987.
[15] R.S. Berns, Principles of Color Technology (http:/ / www. wiley. com/ WileyCDA/ WileyTitle/ productCd-047119459X. html), 3rd Ed.,
Wiley, New York (2001).
Color 11
Color Theory
Color space
A color model is an abstract mathematical model describing the way
colors can be represented as tuples of numbers, typically as three or
four values or color components (e.g. RGB and CMYK are color
models). However, a color model with no associated mapping function
to an absolute color space is a more or less arbitrary color system with
no connection to any globally understood system of color
interpretation.
In the most generic sense of the definition above, color spaces can be
defined without the use of a color model. These spaces, such as Pantone, are in effect a given set of names or
numbers which are defined by the existence of a corresponding set of physical color swatches. This article focuses
on the mathematical model concept.
However, this is not the only possible color space. For instance, when
colors are displayed on a computer monitor, they are usually defined in
the RGB (red, green and blue) color space. This is another way of
making nearly the same colors (limited by the reproduction medium, A comparison of RGB and CMYK color models.
This image demonstrates the difference between
such as the phosphor (CRT) or filters and backlight (LCD)), and red,
how colors will look on a computer monitor
green and blue can be considered as the X, Y and Z axes. Another way (RGB) compared to how they will reproduce in a
of making the same colors is to use their Hue (X axis), their Saturation CMYK print process.
(Y axis), and their brightness Value (Z axis). This is called the HSV
color space. Many color spaces can be represented as three-dimensional (X,Y,Z) values in this manner, but some
have more, or fewer dimensions, and some, such as Pantone, cannot be represented in this way at all.
Color space 13
Notes
When formally defining a color space, the usual reference standard is the CIELAB or CIEXYZ color spaces, which
were specifically designed to encompass all colors the average human can see.
Since "color space" is a more specific term for a certain combination of a color model plus a mapping function, the
term "color space" tends to be used to also identify color models, since identifying a color space automatically
identifies the associated color model. Informally, the two terms are often used interchangeably, though this is strictly
incorrect. For example, although several specific color spaces are based on the RGB model, there is no such thing as
the RGB color space.
Since any color space defines colors as a function of the absolute reference frame, color spaces, along with device
profiling, allow reproducible representations of color, in both analogue and digital representations.
Conversion
Color space conversion is the translation of the representation of a color from one basis to another. This typically
occurs in the context of converting an image that is represented in one color space to another color space, the goal
being to make the translated image look as similar as possible to the original.
Density
The RGB color model is implemented in different ways, depending on the capabilities of the system used. By far the
most common general-used incarnation as of 2006 is the 24-bit implementation, with 8 bits, or 256 discrete levels of
color per channel. Any color space based on such a 24-bit RGB model is thus limited to a range of 256×256×256 ≈
16.7 million colors. Some implementations use 16 bits per component for 48 bits total, resulting in the same gamut
with a larger number of distinct colors. This is especially important when working with wide-gamut color spaces
(where most of the more common colors are located relatively close together), or when a large number of digital
filtering algorithms are used consecutively. The same principle applies for any color space based on the same color
model, but implemented in different bit depths.
Color space 14
brightness of white, while the lightness of a pure color is equal to the lightness of a medium gray.
References
[1] William David Wright, 50 years of the 1931 CIE Standard Observer. Die Farbe, 29:4/6 (1981).
[2] Dean Anderson. "Color Spaces in Frame Grabbers: RGB vs. YUV" (http:/ / www. sensoray. com/ support/ frame_grabber_capture_modes.
htm). . Retrieved 2008-04-08.
External links
• Color FAQ (http://www.poynton.com/ColorFAQ.html), Charles Poynton
• FAQ about color physics (http://www.colourware.co.uk/cpfaq.htm), Stephen Westland
• Color Science (http://www.physics.sfasu.edu/astro/color.html), Dan Bruton
• Color Spaces (http://www4.ncsu.edu/~rgkuehni/PDFs/ColSp.pdf), Rolf G. Kuehni (October 2003)
• Colour spaces – perceptual, historical and applicational background (http://ldos.fe.uni-lj.si/docs/documents/
20030929092037_markot.pdf), Marko Tkalčič (2003)
• Color formats (http://www.equasys.de/colorformat.html) for image and video processing – Color conversion
(http://www.equasys.de/colorconversion.html) between RGB, YUV, YCbCr and YPbPr.
• C library (http://pixfc-sse.googlecode.com) of SSE-optimised color format conversions.
• Konica Minolta Sensing: Precise Color Communication (http://www2.konicaminolta.eu/eu/Measuring/pcc/
en/index.html)
Color theory 16
Color theory
In the visual arts, color theory is a body of practical guidance to color mixing and the visual effects of specific color
combination. There are also definitions (or categories) of colors based on the color wheel: primary color, secondary
color and tertiary color. Although color theory principles first appeared in the writings of Leone Battista Alberti
(c.1435) and the notebooks of Leonardo da Vinci (c.1490), a tradition of "colory theory" began in the 18th century,
initially within a partisan controversy around Isaac Newton's theory of color (Opticks, 1704) and the nature of
so-called primary colors. From there it developed as an independent artistic tradition with only superficial reference
to colorimetry and vision science.
Color abstractions
The foundations of pre-20th-century color theory were built around "pure" or ideal colors, characterized by sensory
experiences rather than attributes of the physical world. This has led to a number of inaccuracies in traditional color
theory principles that are not always remedied in modern formulations.
The most important problem has been a confusion between the behavior of light mixtures, called additive color, and
the behavior of paint or ink or dye or pigment mixtures, called subtractive color. This problem arises because the
absorption of light by material substances follows different rules from the perception of light by the eye.
A second problem has been the failure to describe the very important effects of strong luminance (lightness)
contrasts in the appearance of colors reflected from a surface (such as paints or inks) as opposed to colors of light;
"colors" such as browns or ochres cannot appear in mixtures of light. Thus, a strong lightness contrast between a
mid-valued yellow paint and a surrounding bright white makes the yellow appear to be green or brown, while a
strong brightness contrast between a rainbow and the surrounding sky makes the yellow in a rainbow appear to be a
fainter yellow, or white.
A third problem has been the tendency to describe color effects holistically or categorically, for example as a
contrast between "yellow" and "blue" conceived as generic colors, when most color effects are due to contrasts on
three relative attributes that define all colors:
1. lightness (light vs. dark, or white vs. black),
2. saturation (intense vs. dull), and
3. hue (e.g., red, orange, yellow, green, blue or purple).
Thus, the visual impact of "yellow" vs. "blue" hues in visual design depends on the relative lightness and intensity of
the hues.
These confusions are partly historical, and arose in scientific uncertainty about color perception that was not resolved
until the late 19th century, when the artistic notions were already entrenched. However, they also arise from the
attempt to describe the highly contextual and flexible behavior of color perception in terms of abstract color
sensations that can be generated equivalently by any visual media.
Many historical "color theorists" have assumed that three "pure" primary colors can mix all possible colors, and that
any failure of specific paints or inks to match this ideal performance is due to the impurity or imperfection of the
Color theory 17
colorants. In reality, only imaginary "primary colors" used in colorimetry can "mix" or quantify all visible
(perceptually possible) colors; but to do this, these imaginary primaries are defined as lying outside the range of
visible colors; i.e., they cannot be seen. Any three real "primary" colors of light, paint or ink can mix only a limited
range of colors, called a gamut, which is always smaller (contains fewer colors) than the full range of colors humans
can perceive.
Historical background
Color theory was originally formulated in terms of three "primary" or "primitive" colors—red, yellow and blue
(RYB)—because these colors were believed capable of mixing all other colors. This color mixing behavior had long
been known to printers, dyers and painters, but these trades preferred pure pigments to primary color mixtures,
because the mixtures were too dull (unsaturated).
The RYB primary colors became the foundation of 18th century
theories of color vision, as the fundamental sensory qualities that are
blended in the perception of all physical colors and equally in the
physical mixture of pigments or dyes. These theories were enhanced by
18th-century investigations of a variety of purely psychological color
effects, in particular the contrast between "complementary" or
opposing hues that are produced by color afterimages and in the
contrasting shadows in colored light. These ideas and many personal
color observations were summarized in two founding documents in
color theory: the Theory of Colours (1810) by the German poet and
government minister Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, and The Law of
Simultaneous Color Contrast (1839) by the French industrial chemist
Michel Eugène Chevreul. Goethe's color wheel from his 1810 Theory of
Colours
Subsequently, German and English scientists established in the late
19th century that color perception is best described in terms of a different set of primary colors—red, green and blue
violet (RGB)—modeled through the additive mixture of three monochromatic lights. Subsequent research anchored
these primary colors in the differing responses to light by three types of color receptors or cones in the retina
(trichromacy). On this basis the quantitative description of color mixture or colorimetry developed in the early 20th
century, along with a series of increasingly sophisticated models of color space and color perception, such as the
opponent process theory.
Across the same period, industrial chemistry radically expanded the color range of lightfast synthetic pigments,
allowing for substantially improved saturation in color mixtures of dyes, paints and inks. It also created the dyes and
chemical processes necessary for color photography. As a result three-color printing became aesthetically and
economically feasible in mass printed media, and the artists' color theory was adapted to primary colors most
effective in inks or photographic dyes: cyan, magenta, and yellow (CMY). (In printing, dark colors are supplemented
by a black ink, known as the CMYK system; in both printing and photography, white is provided by the color of the
paper.) These CMY primary colors were reconciled with the RGB primaries, and subtractive color mixing with
additive color mixing, by defining the CMY primaries as substances that absorbed only one of the retinal primary
colors: cyan absorbs only red (−R+G+B), magenta only green (+R−G+B), and yellow only blue violet (+R+G−B). It
is important to add that the CMYK, or process, color printing is meant as an economical way of producing a wide
range of colors for printing, but is deficient in reproducing certain colors, notably orange and slightly deficient in
reproducing purples. A wider range of color can be obtained with the addition of other colors to the printing process,
such as in Pantone's Hexachrome printing ink system (six colors), among others.
Color theory 18
For much of the 19th century artistic color theory either lagged behind
scientific understanding or was augmented by science books written
for the lay public, in particular Modern Chromatics (1879) by the
American physicist Ogden Rood, and early color atlases developed by
Albert Munsell (Munsell Book of Color, 1915, see Munsell color
system) and Wilhelm Ostwald (Color Atlas, 1919). Major advances
were made in the early 20th century by artists teaching or associated
with the German Bauhaus, in particular Wassily Kandinsky, Johannes
Itten, Faber Birren and Josef Albers, whose writings mix speculation
with an empirical or demonstration-based study of color design
principles.
Contemporary color theory must address the expanded range of media Munsell's color system represented as a
three-dimensional solid showing all three color
created by digital media and print management systems, which
making attributes: lightness, saturation and hue.
substantially expand the range of imaging systems and viewing
contexts in which color can be used. These applications are areas of
intensive research, much of it proprietary; artistic color theory has little to say about these complex new
opportunities.
Complementary colors
For the mixing of colored light, Newton's
color wheel is often used to describe
complementary colors, which are colors
which cancel each other's hue to produce an
achromatic (white, gray or black) light
mixture. Newton offered as a conjecture that
colors exactly opposite one another on the
hue circle cancel out each other's hue; this
concept was demonstrated more thoroughly
in the 19th century.
One reason the artist's primary colors even work at all is that the imperfect pigments being used have sloped
absorption curves, and thus change color with concentration. A pigment that is pure red at high concentrations can
behave more like magenta at low concentrations. This allows it to make purples that would otherwise be impossible.
Likewise, a blue that is ultramarine at high concentrations appears cyan at low concentrations, allowing it to be used
to mix green. Chromium red pigments can appear orange, and then yellow, as the concentration is reduced. It is even
possible to mix very low concentrations of the blue mentioned and the chromium red to get a greenish color. This
works much better with oil colors than it does with water colors and dyes.
So the old primaries depend on sloped absorption curves and pigment leakages to work, while the new scientifically
derived ones depend solely on controlling the amount of absorption in certain parts of the spectrum.
Another reason the correct primary colors were not used by early artists is that they were not available as durable
pigments. Modern methods in chemistry were needed to produce them.
The hottest radiating bodies (e.g. stars) have a "cool" color while the less hot bodies radiate with a "warm" color. (Image in mired scale.)
Achromatic colors
Any color that lacks strong chromatic content is said to be unsaturated, achromatic, or near neutral. Pure achromatic
colors include black, white and all grays; near neutrals include browns, tans, pastels and darker colors. Near neutrals
can be of any hue or lightness.
Neutrals are obtained by mixing pure colors with white, black or grey, or by mixing two complementary colors. In
color theory, neutral colors are colors easily modified by adjacent more saturated colors and they appear to take on
the hue complementary to the saturated color. Next to a bright red couch, a gray wall will appear distinctly greenish.
Black and white have long been known to combine well with almost any other colors; black increases the apparent
saturation or brightness of colors paired with it, and white shows off all hues to equal effect.
Wherein color harmony is a function (f) of the interaction between color/s (Col 1, 2, 3, …, n) and the factors that
influence positive aesthetic response to color: individual differences (ID) such as age, gender, personality and
affective state; cultural experiences (CE), the prevailing context (CX) which includes setting and ambient lighting;
intervening perceptual effects (P) and the effects of time (T) in terms of prevailing social trends.[3]
In addition, given that humans can perceive over 2.8 million different hues,[4] it has been suggested that the number
of possible color combinations is virtually infinite thereby implying that predictive color harmony formulae are
fundamentally unsound.[5] Despite this, many color theorists have devised formulae, principles or guidelines for
color combination with the aim being to predict or specify positive aesthetic response or "color harmony". Color
wheel models have often been used as a basis for color combination principles or guidelines and for defining
relationships between colors. Some theorists and artists believe juxtapositions of complementary color will produce
strong contrast, a sense of visual tension as well as "color harmony"; while others believe juxtapositions of
analogous colors will elicit positive aesthetic response. Color combination guidelines suggest that colors next to each
other on the color wheel model (analogous colors) tend to produce a single-hued or monochromatic color experience
and some theorists also refer to these as "simple harmonies". In addition, split complementary color schemes usually
depict a range of analogous hues plus a key complementary color. A triadic color scheme adopts any three colors
approximately equidistant around a color wheel model. Feisner and Mahnke are among a number of authors who
provide color combination guidelines in greater detail.[6][7]
Color combination formulae and principles may provide some guidance but have limited practical application. This
is because of the influence of contextual, perceptual and temporal factors which will influence how color/s are
perceived in any given situation, setting or context. Such formulae and principles may be useful in fashion, interior
and graphic design, but much depends on the tastes, lifestyle and cultural norms of the viewer or consumer.
As early as the ancient Greek philosophers, many theorists have devised color associations and linked particular
connotative meanings to specific colors. However, connotative color associations and color symbolism tends to be
culture-bound and may also vary across different contexts and circumstances. For example, red has many different
connotative and symbolic meanings from exciting, arousing, sensual, romantic and feminine; to a symbol of good
Color theory 22
luck; and also acts as a signal of danger. Such color associations tend to be learned and do not necessarily hold
irrespective of individual and cultural differences or contextual, temporal or perceptual factors.[8] It is important to
note that while color symbolism and color associations exist, their existence does not provide evidential support for
color psychology or claims that color has therapeutic properties.[9]
Current status
Color theory has not developed an explicit explanation of how specific media affect color appearance: colors have
always been defined in the abstract, and whether the colors were inks or paints, oils or watercolors, transparencies or
reflecting prints, computer displays or movie theaters, was not considered especially relevant. Josef Albers
investigated the effects of relative contrast and color saturation on the illusion of transparency, but this is an
exception to the rule.[10] Known for his color theory paintings of the 1970's, Frederick Spratt created diptychs
consisting of flat rectangular panels of two different monochrome colors. [11]
References
[1] "color temperature" (http:/ / www. handprint. com/ HP/ WCL/ color12. html). handprint. 2009-04-19. . Retrieved 2011-06-09.
[2] Burchett, K. E. (2002). Color harmony. Color Research and Application, 27 (1), pp28-31.
[3] O'Connor, Z. (2010). Color harmony revisited. Color Research and Application, 35 (4), pp267-273.
[4] Pointer, M. R. & Attridge, G.G. (1998). The number of discernible colors. Color Research and Application, 23 (1), pp52-54.
[5] Hard, A. & Sivik, L. (2001). A theory of colors in combination - A descriptive model related to the NCS color-order system. Color Research
and Application, 26 (1), pp4-28.
[6] Feisner, E. A. (2000). Colour: How to use colour in art and design. London: Laurence King.
[7] Mahnke, F. (1996). Color, environment and human response. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
[8] Bellantoni, Patti (2005). If it's Purple, Someone's Gonna Die. Elsevier, Focal Press. ISBN 0-240-80688-3.
[9] O'Connor, Z. (2010). Colour psychology and colour therapy: Caveat emptor. Color Research and Application, (Published online in
'EarlyView' in advance of print).
[10] Albers, Josef (2006). Interaction of Color. Revised and Expanded Edition. Yale University Press. ISBN 0-300-11595-4.
[11] http:/ / artshiftsanjose. com/ ?p=440
External links
• Color Theory Tutorial by Worqx (http://www.worqx.com/color/)
• Handprint.com : Color (http://handprint.com/HP/WCL/wcolor.html) - a comprehensive site about color
perception, color psychology, color theory, and color mixing
• Color Theory in Landscape Design (http://landscaping.about.com/od/flowersherbsgroundcover1/a/
flower_photos.htm)
• The Dimensions of Colour (http://www.huevaluechroma.com/) - color theory for artists using digital/
traditional media
• Color Thesaurus (http://www.hpl.hp.com/personal/Nathan_Moroney/color-thesaurus.html) World's Largest
Database of Color Names
• Stanford University CS 178 interactive Flash demo (http://graphics.stanford.edu/courses/cs178/applets/locus.
html) introducing trichromatic color theory.
• App that generates harmonious color palettes from photos based on color theory (http://realcolors.
makan-studios.com)
Additive color 23
Additive color
Additive color is color created by mixing together light of two or more
different colors. Red, green, and blue are the additive primary colors
normally used in additive color system. Additive color is in contrast to
subtractive color, in which colors are created by subtracting
(absorbing) parts of the spectrum of light present in ordinary white
light, by means of colored pigments or dyes, such as those in paints,
inks, and the three dye layers in typical color photographs on film.
History
Systems of additive color are motivated by the Young–Helmholtz
theory of trichromatic color vision, which was articulated around 1850
by Hermann von Helmholtz, based on earlier work by Thomas Young.
For his experimental work on the subject, James Clerk Maxwell is
sometimes credited as being the father of additive color.[2] He had the
photographer Thomas Sutton photograph a tartan ribbon on
black-and-white film three times, first with a red, then green, then blue
color filter over the lens. The three black-and-white images were
developed and then projected onto a screen with three different
projectors, each equipped with the corresponding red, green, or blue
The first permanent color photograph, taken by
color filter used to take its image. When brought into alignment, the James Clerk Maxwell in 1861.
three images (a black-and-red image, a black-and-green image and a
black-and-blue image) formed a full color image, thus demonstrating the principles of additive color.[3]
Examples
The following flowchart demonstrates an example of the process, step
by step.
Light source Medium wavelengths, or green, and long wavelengths, or red, radiate from two different projectors.
Projection screen Both the medium and long wavelengths reflect off of a spot on the screen.
Retina The medium and long wavelengths activate M and L cones on a spot on the retina.
Brain The brain interprets the equal amounts of medium and long signal as yellow.
To fully understand the process, it should be demonstrated how dull colors are obtained using cyan, magenta, and
yellow instead of red, green, and blue.
Additive color 25
Light source Cyan, or SM, and yellow, or ML, radiate from two different projectors.
Retina Some short, lots of medium, and some long wavelengths activate cones on a spot on the retina.
Brain The brain receives signals from the cones about some short, lots of medium, and some long wavelengths. It interprets the signal
as light green.
If the background is not black, it interprets the signal as dull green.
References
[1] David Briggs (2007). "The Dimensions of Color" (http:/ / www. huevaluechroma. com/ 044. php). . Retrieved 2011-11-23.
[2] "James Clerk Maxwell" (http:/ / www. cis. rit. edu/ node/ 280). Inventor's Hall of Fame, Rochester Institute of Technology Center for
Imaging Science. .
[3] Robert Hirsch (2004). Exploring Colour Photography: A Complete Guide (http:/ / books. google. com/ books?id=4Gx2WItWGYoC&
pg=PA28& dq=maxwell+ additive+ color+ photograph+ register#PPA28,M1). Laurence King Publishing. ISBN 1-85669-420-8. .
External links
• http://www.edinphoto.org.uk/1_P/1_photographers_maxwell.htm (http://www.edinphoto.org.uk/1_P/
1_photographers_maxwell.htm) - Photos and stories from the James Clerk Maxwell Foundation.
• Stanford University CS 178 interactive Flash demo (http://graphics.stanford.edu/courses/cs178/applets/
colormixing.html) comparing additive and subtractive color mixing.
Subtractive color
A subtractive color model explains the mixing of a limited set of
dyes, inks, paint pigments or natural colorants to create a wider range
of colors, each the result of partially or completely subtracting (that is,
absorbing) some wavelengths of light and not others. The color that a
surface displays depends on which parts of the visible spectrum are not
absorbed and therefore remain visible.
RYB
RYB (Red, Yellow, Blue) is the formerly standard set of subtractive
primary colors used for mixing pigments. It is used in art and art
education, particularly in painting. It predated modern scientific color
theory.
Red, yellow, and blue are the primary colors of the standard color
"wheel". The secondary colors, violet (or purple), orange, and green
(VOG) make up another triad, formed by mixing equal amounts of red
and blue, red and yellow, and blue and yellow, respectively.
The RYB primary colors became the foundation of 18th century
theories of color vision as the fundamental sensory qualities blended in
the perception of all physical colors and equally in the physical mixture
Standard RYB Color Wheel
of pigments or dyes. These theories were enhanced by 18th-century
investigations of a variety of purely psychological color effects, in
particular the contrast between "complementary" or opposing hues produced by color afterimages and in the
contrasting shadows in colored light. These ideas and many personal color observations were summarized in two
founding documents in color theory: the Theory of Colors (1810) by the German poet and government minister
Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, and The Law of Simultaneous Color Contrast (1839) by the French industrial chemist
Michel-Eugène Chevreul.
In late 19th and early to mid-20th century commercial printing, use of the traditional RYB terminology persisted
even though the more versatile CMY (Cyan, Magenta, Yellow) triad had been adopted, with the cyan sometimes
referred to as "process blue" and the magenta as "process red".
In inkjet color printing and typical mass production photomechanical printing processes, a black ink K (Key)
component is included, resulting in the CMYK color model. The black ink serves to cover unwanted tints in dark
areas of the printed image, which result from the imperfect transparency of commercially practical CMY inks; to
improve image sharpness, which tends to be degraded by imperfect registration of the three color elements; and to
reduce or eliminate consumption of the more expensive color inks where only black or gray is required.
Purely photographic color processes almost never include a K component, because in all common processes the
CMY dyes used are much more perfectly transparent, there are no registration errors to camouflage, and substituting
a black dye for a saturated CMY combination, a trivial prospective cost benefit at best, is technologically impractical
in non-electronic analog photography.
References
• Berns, Roy S. (2000). Billmeyer and Saltzman's Principles of Color Technology, 3rd edition. Wiley, New York.
ISBN 0-471-19459-X.
• Stroebel, Leslie, John Compton, Ira Current, and Richard Zakia (2000). Basic Photographic Materials and
Processes, 2nd edition. Focal Press, Boston. ISBN 0-240-80405-8.
• Wyszecki, Günther and W. S. Stiles (1982). Colour Science: Concept and Methods, Quantitative Data and
Formulae. Wiley, New York. ISBN 0-471-02106-7.
External links
• Stanford University CS 178 interactive Flash demo [1] comparing additive and subtractive color mixing.
References
[1] http:/ / graphics. stanford. edu/ courses/ cs178/ applets/ colormixing. html
28
Mixing Color
Color mixing
There are two types of color mixing: Additive and Subtractive. In both
cases there are three primary colors, three secondary colors (colors
made from 2 of the three primary colors in equal amounts), and one
tertiary color made from all three primary colors.
Additive Mixing
Additive mixing of colors generally involves mixing colors of light. In
additive mixing of colors there are three primary colors: red, green, and
blue. In the absence of color or, when no colors are showing, the result
is black. If all three primary colors are showing, the result is white.
When red and green combine, the result is yellow. When red and blue
combine, the result is magenta. When blue and green combine, the
result is cyan. Additive mixing is used in television and computer
A simulated example of additive color mixing monitors to produce a wide range of colors using only three primary
colors.
Subtractive Mixing
Subtractive mixing is done by selectively removing certain colors, for
instance with optical filters. The three primary colors in subtractive
mixing are yellow, magenta, and cyan. In subtractive mixing of color,
the absence of color is white and the presence of all three primary
colors is black. In subtractive mixing of colors, the secondary colors
are the same as the primary colors from additive mixing, and vice
versa. Subtractive mixing is used to create a variety of colors when
printing on paper by combining a small number of ink colors, and also
when painting. The mixing of pigments does not produce perfect
subtractive color mixing because some light from the subtracted color
A simulated example of subtractive color mixing
is still being reflected. This results in a darker and desaturated color
compared to the color that would be achieved with ideal filters.
Color mixing 29
Importance to vision
Additive color mixing—red and green combining to make yellow, for example, or blue and yellow producing
white—runs counter to the commonsense observation that, for example, yellow paint plus cyan paint makes green
paint. In this case, one must understand that the wavelengths of light that reach the eye are often selected via these
more intuitive subtractive processes: for example, cyan paint appears to our eye as cyan because it absorbs red
wavelengths, and a yellow paint appears yellow because it absorbs blue wavelengths. When white light falls on a
combination of cyan and yellow, then, both red and blue are absorbed, and green is reflected to the eye.[1]
External links
• Interactive Java applet on the additive mixing of RGB colors [2] by Wolfgang Bauer
• Interactive Java applet on the subtractive mixing of CYM colors [3] by Wolfgang Bauer
• Online Color Mixing Tool for Designers/ Artists [4]
References
• Macaulay, David and Neil Ardley (1988). The New Way Things Work. London: Dorling Kindersley Ltd. ISBN
0-395-93847-3.
[1] "Sensory Reception: Human Vision: Structure and Function of the Eye" Encyclopaedia Britannica, vol. 27 1987
[2] http:/ / chair. pa. msu. edu/ applets/ RGBColor/ a. htm
[3] http:/ / chair. pa. msu. edu/ applets/ CYMColor/ a. htm
[4] http:/ / knowpapa. com/ cmt/
Primary color
Primary colours are sets of colours
that can be combined to make a useful
range of colours. For human
applications, three primary colours are
usually used, since human colour
vision is trichromatic.
Any particular choice for a given set of primary colours is derived from the spectral sensitivity of each of the human
cone photoreceptors; three colours that fall within each of the sensitivity ranges of each of the human cone cells are
red, green, and blue.[3] Other sets of colours can be used, though not all will well approximate the full range of
Primary color 30
colour perception. For example, an early colour photographic process, autochrome, typically used orange, green, and
violet primaries.[4] However, unless negative amounts of a colour are allowed the gamut will be restricted by the
choice of primaries.
The combination of any two primary colours creates a secondary colour.
The most commonly used additive colour primaries are the secondary colours of the most commonly used
subtractive colour primaries, and vice versa.
Biological basis
Primary colours are not a fundamental property of light but are related
to the physiological response of the eye to light.Fundamentally, light is
a continuous spectrum of the wavelengths that can be detected by the
human eye, an infinite-dimensional stimulus space.[5] However, the
human eye normally contains only three types of colour receptors,
called cone cells. Each colour receptor responds to different ranges of
the colour spectrum. Humans and other species with three such types
of colour receptors are known as trichromats. These species respond to
the light stimulus via a three-dimensional sensation, which generally
RGB lasers
can be modeled as a mixture of three primary colours.[5]
Before the nature of colourimetry and visual physiology were well understood, scientists such as Thomas Young,
James Clark Maxwell, and Hermann von Helmholtz expressed various opinions about what should be the three
primary colours to describe the three primary colour sensations of the eye.[6] Young originally proposed red, green,
and violet, and Maxwell changed violet to blue; Helmholtz proposed "a slightly purplish red, a vegetation-green,
slightly yellowish (wave-length about 5600 tenth-metres), and an ultramarine-blue (about 4820)".[7] In modern
understanding, the human cone cells do not correspond to any real primary colours.
Species with different numbers of receptor cell types would have colour vision requiring a different number of
primaries. For example, for species known as tetrachromats, with four different colour receptors, one would use four
primary colours. Since humans can only see to 380 nanometers (violet), but tetrachromats can see into the ultraviolet
to about 300 nanometers, this fourth primary colour for tetrachromats is located in the shorter-wavelength range.[8]
Many birds and marsupials are tetrachromats, and it has been suggested that some human females are tetrachromats
as well,[9][10] having an extra variant version of the long-wave (L) cone type.[11] The peak response of human colour
receptors varies, even among individuals with "normal" colour vision;[12] in non-human species this polymorphic
variation is even greater, and it may well be adaptive.[13] Most placental mammals other than primates have only two
types of colour receptors and are therefore dichromats; to them, there are only two primary colours.
It would be incorrect to assume that the world "looks tinted" to an animal (or human) with anything other than the
human standard of three colour receptors. To an animal (or human) born that way, the world would look normal to it,
but the animal's ability to detect and discriminate colours would be different from that of a human with normal
colour vision. If a human and an animal both look at a natural colour, they see it as natural; however, if both look at a
colour reproduced via primary colours, such as on a colour television screen, the human may see it as matching the
natural colour, while the animal does not, since the primary colours have been chosen to suit human capabilities.
Primary color 31
Additive primaries
Media that combine emitted lights to create the sensation of a range of
colours are using the additive colour system. Typically, the primary
colours used are red, green, and blue.[14]
Television and other computer and video displays are a common
example of the use of additive primaries and the RGB colour model.
The exact colours chosen for the primaries are a technological
compromise between the available phosphors (including considerations
such as cost and power usage) and the need for large colour triangle to
allow a large gamut of colours. The ITU-R BT.709-5/sRGB primaries
are typical.
never stored. Regardless, industry is still exploring a wide variety of "primary" active light sources (per pixel) with
the goal of matching the capability of human colour perception within a broadly affordable price. One example of a
potentially affordable, but yet unproven active light hybrid places a LED screen over a plasma light screen, each
with different "primaries". Because both LED and plasma technologies are many decades old (plasma pixels going
back to the 1960s) and because sales are verging on a billion, both have become so affordable that they could be
combined.
Subtractive primaries
Media that use reflected light and colourants to produce colours are using the subtractive colour method of colour
mixing.
History
Painters have long used more than three "primary" colours in their palettes—and at one point considered red, yellow,
blue, and green to be the four primaries.[25] Red, yellow, blue, and green are still widely considered the four
psychological primary colours,[26] though red, yellow, and blue are sometimes listed as the three psychological
primaries,[27] with black and white occasionally added as a fourth and fifth.[28]
During the 18th century, as theorists became aware of Isaac Newton's scientific experiments with light and prisms,
red, yellow, and blue became the canonical primary colours—supposedly the fundamental sensory qualities that are
blended in the perception of all physical colours and equally in the physical mixture of pigments or dyes. This theory
became dogma, despite abundant evidence that red, yellow, and blue primaries cannot mix all other colours, and has
survived in colour theory to the present day.[29]
Using red, yellow, and blue as primaries yields a relatively small gamut, in which, among other problems, colourful
greens, cyans, and magentas are impossible to mix, because red, yellow, and blue are not well-spaced around a
perceptually uniform colour wheel. For this reason, modern three- or four-colour printing processes, as well as
colour photography, use cyan, yellow, and magenta as primaries instead.[30] Most painters include colours in their
palettes which cannot be mixed from yellow, red, and blue paints, and thus do not fit within the RYB colour model.
Some who do use a three-colour palette opt for the more evenly spaced cyan, yellow, and magenta used by printers,
and others paint with 6 or more colours to widen their gamuts.[31] The cyan, magenta, and yellow used in printing are
sometimes known as "process blue," "process red," and "process yellow."[32]
Psychological primaries
Main article: Opponent process. See also: Natural Colour System, Unique hues
The opponent process is a colour theory that states that the human visual system interprets information about colour
by processing signals from cones and rods in an antagonistic manner. The three types of cones have some overlap in
the wavelengths of light to which they respond, so it is more efficient for the visual system to record differences
between the responses of cones, rather than each type of cone's individual response. The opponent colour theory
suggests that there are three opponent channels: red versus green, blue versus yellow, and black versus white.[33]
Responses to one colour of an opponent channel are antagonistic to those of the other colour. The particular colours
considered by an observer to be uniquely representative of the concepts red, yellow, green, blue, white, and black
might be called "psychological primary colours", because any other colour can be described in terms of some
combination of these.
[10] Pr. Mollon (Cambridge university), Pr. Jordan (Newcastle university) "Study of women heterozygote for colour difficiency" (Vision
Research, 1993)
[11] M. Neitz, T. W. Kraft, and J. Neitz (1998). "Expression of L cone pigment gene subtypes in females". Vision Research 38 (21): 3221–3225.
doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00076-5. PMID 9893829.
[12] Neitz, Jay & Jacobs, Gerald H. (1986). "Polymorphism of the long-wavelength cone in normal human colour vision." (http:/ / www. nature.
com/ nature/ journal/ v323/ n6089/ abs/ 323623a0. html) Nature. 323, 623–625.
[13] Jacobs, Gerald H. (1996). "Primate photopigments and primate colour vision." (http:/ / www. pubmedcentral. nih. gov/ articlerender.
fcgi?artid=40094) PNAS. 93 (2), 577–581.
[14] Thomas D. Rossing and Christopher J. Chiaverina (1999). Light science: physics and the visual arts (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=jpH1_dCT_UcC& pg=PA178& dq=red+ green+ blue+ additive+ colour+ primaries+ violet). Birkhäuser. p. 178.
ISBN 978-0-387-98827-6. .
[15] "Some Experiments on Colour", Nature 111, 1871, in John William Strutt (Lord Rayleigh) (1899). Scientific Papers (http:/ / books. google.
com/ ?id=KWMSAAAAIAAJ& pg=PA84& dq=date:0-1923+ light+ red+ green+ yellow-or-orange). University Press. .
[16] Garvey, Jude (2010-01-20). "Sharp four primary colour TVs enable over one trillion colours" (http:/ / www. gizmag. com/
sharp-4-primary-colour-tvs-enables-trillion-colours/ 13823/ ). www.gizmag.com. .
[17] M. R. Pointer (1980). "The Gamut of Real Surface Colours" (http:/ / onlinelibrary. wiley. com/ doi/ 10. 1002/ col. 5080050308/ abstract)
(PDF, pay per view). Colour Research and Application (John Wiley & Sons, Inc.) 5 (3): 145–155. doi:10.1002/col.5080050308. .
[18] Chih-Cheng Chan (1999) (PDF, Industrial research report). Development of Multi-Primary Colour LCD (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=jpH1_dCT_UcC& pg=PA178& dq=red+ green+ blue+ additive+ colour+ primaries+ violet). AU Optronics, Science-Based Industrial
Park, Hsin-Chu, Taiwan; Genoa Colour Technologies, Herzelia, Israel. .
[19] Abhinav Priya (2011-07) (PDF, academic literature review), Five-Primary Colour LCD (http:/ / www. scribd. com/ doc/ 61823459/
Five-Primary-Colour-LCD), Cochin University of Science and Technology, Department of Electronics Engineering, p. 2,
[20] Ervin Sidney Ferry (1921). General Physics and Its Application to Industry and Everyday Life (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=3rYXAAAAIAAJ& pg=PA621& dq=date:0-1923+ additive+ colour+ mixing+ primary). John Wiley & Sons. .
[21] Frank S. Henry (1917). Printing for School and Shop: A Textbook for Printers' Apprentices, Continuation Classes, and for General use in
Schools (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=UAAvAAAAMAAJ& pg=PA292& dq=black+ date:0-1923+ key-plate+ printing+ colour). John
Wiley & Sons. .
[22] See the google image results (http:/ / images. google. com/ images?q=cmyk gamut) for "cmyk gamut" for examples.
[23] Tom Fraser and Adam Banks (2004). Designer's Colour Manual: The Complete Guide to Colour Theory and Application (http:/ / books.
google. com/ ?id=WXZNPaX-LvcC& pg=PA27& dq=red-yellow-blue+ colour+ mixing). Chronicle Books. ISBN 0-8118-4210-X. .
[24] Stephen Quiller (2002). Colour Choices (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=jiUTZQj_v5QC& pg=PA12& dq=what-is-a-colour-wheel+
spaced+ red+ yellow+ blue). Watson–Guptill. ISBN 0-8230-0697-2. .
[25] For instance Leonardo da Vinci wrote of these four simple colours in his notebook circa 1500. See Rolf Kuenhi. "Development of the Idea
of Simple Colours in the 16th and Early 17th Centuries". Colour Research and Application. Volume 32, Number 2, April 2007.
[26] Resultby Leslie D. Stroebel, Ira B. Current (2000). Basic Photographic Materials and Processes (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=BRYa6Qpsw48C& pg=PP1& dq=Basic+ Photographic+ Materials+ and+ Processes). Focal Press. ISBN 0-240-80345-0. .
[27] MS Sharon Ross , Elise Kinkead (2004). Decorative Painting & Faux Finishes (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=DPJUWRydR9kC&
dq=red+ yellow+ blue+ paint-mixing+ + subtractive). Creative Homeowner. ISBN 1-58011-179-3. .
[28] Swirnoff, Lois (2003). Dimensional Colour (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=sG5MqtZuFF0C& dq="psychological+ primaries"+ blue+
-green). W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 0-393-73102-2. .
[29] Bruce MacEvoy. "Do 'Primary' Colours Exist?" ( Material Trichromacy section (http:/ / www. handprint. com/ HP/ WCL/ colour6.
html#materialtrichromacy)). Handprint. Accessed 10 August 2007.
[30] "Development of the Idea of Simple Colours in the 16th and Early 17th Centuries". Colour Research and Application. Volume 32, Number
2, April 2007.
[31] Bruce MacEvoy. " Secondary Palette (http:/ / www. handprint. com/ HP/ WCL/ palette4e. html)." Handprint. Accessed 14 August 2007. For
general discussion see Bruce MacEvoy. "Mixing With a Colour Wheel" ( Saturation Costs section (http:/ / www. handprint. com/ HP/ WCL/
colour14. html#satcost)). Handprint. Accessed 14 August 2007.
[32] Cheap Brochure Printing – Process Blue / Process Red / Process Yellow / Process Black (http:/ / www. printoutlet. us/ glossary.
php?glossaryid=2526)
[33] Michael Foster (1891). A Text-book of physiology (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=Swn8ztLFTdkC& pg=RA1-PA921& dq=hering+
red-green+ yellow-blue+ young-helmholtz+ date:0-1923). Lea Bros. & Co. p. 921. .
Primary color 35
External links
• Bruce MacEvoy. "Do Primary Colours Exist?" (http://www.handprint.com/HP/WCL/colour6.html).
handprint.com. The history and science of primary colours, part of MacEvoy's sprawling comprehensive site
about colour.
• Ask A Scientist: Primary Colours (http://www.newton.dep.anl.gov/askasci/phy00/phy00871.htm)
• The Colour-Sensitive Cones at HyperPhysics (http://hyperphysics.phy-astr.gsu.edu/hbase/vision/colcon.
html#c1)
• Colour Tutorial (http://www.tomjewett.com/colours/index.html)
Colorfulness
HSL saturation increased 50%; notice that changing HSL saturation also affects the perceived lightness of a color
In colorimetry and color theory, colorfulness, chroma, and saturation are related but distinct concepts referring to
the perceived intensity of a specific color. Colorfulness is the degree of difference between a color and gray. Chroma
is the colorfulness relative to the brightness of another color that appears white under similar viewing conditions.
Colorfulness 36
Saturation is the colorfulness of a color relative to its own brightness.[1] Though this general concept is intuitive,
terms such as chroma, saturation, purity, and intensity are often used without great precision, and even when
well-defined depend greatly on the specific color model in use.
A highly colorful stimulus is vivid and intense, while a less colorful stimulus appears more muted, closer to gray.
With no colorfulness at all, a color is a “neutral” gray (an image with no colorfulness in any of its colors is called
grayscale). With three attributes—colorfulness (or chroma or saturation), lightness (or brightness), and hue—any
color can be described.
Saturation
Saturation is one of three coordinates in the HSL and HSV color spaces. Note that virtually all
computer software implementing these spaces use a very rough approximation to calculate the
value they call "saturation", such as the formula described for HSV and this value has little, if
anything, to do with the description shown here.
The saturation of a color is determined by a combination of light intensity and how much it is
distributed across the spectrum of different wavelengths. The purest (most saturated) color is
Scale of
achieved by using just one wavelength at a high intensity, such as in laser light. If the intensity
saturation (0%
at bottom and it drops, then as a result the saturation drops. To desaturate a color of given intensity in a subtractive
is black and system (such as watercolor), one can add white, black, gray, or the hue's complement.
white).
where (u′n, v′n) is the chromaticity of the white point, and chroma is defined below.[2]
By analogy, in CIELAB this would yield:
The CIE has not formally recommended this equation since CIELAB has no chromaticity diagram, and this
definition therefore lacks direct correlation with older concepts of saturation.[3] Nevertheless, this equation provides
a reasonable predictor of saturation, and demonstrates that adjusting the lightness in CIELAB while holding (a*, b*)
fixed does affect the saturation.
But the following formula is in agreement with the human perception of saturation: The formula proposed by Eva
Lübbe is in agreement with the verbal definition of Manfred Richter: Saturation is the proportion of pure chromatic
color in the total color sensation.[4]
where Sab is the saturation, L* the lightness and C*ab is the chroma of the color.
CIECAM02
The square root of the colorfulness divided by the brightness:
Colorfulness 37
This definition is inspired by experimental work done with the intention of remedying CIECAM97s's poor
performance.[5][6] M is proportional to the chroma C (M = CFL0.25), thus the CIECAM02 definition bears some
similarity to the CIELUV definition. An important difference is that the CIECAM02 model accounts for the viewing
conditions through the parameter FL.[5]
Excitation purity
The excitation purity (purity for short) of a
stimulus is its difference from the
illuminant's white point relative to the
furthest point on the chromaticity diagram
with the same hue (dominant wavelength for
monochromatic sources); using the CIE
1931 color space:[7]
In the CIE 1976 L*a*b* and L*u*v* color spaces, the unnormalized chroma is the radial component of the
cylindrical coordinate CIE L*C*h (lightness, chroma, hue) representation of the L*a*b* and L*u*v* color spaces,
also denoted as CIE L*C*h(a*b*) or CIE L*C*h for short, and CIE L*C*h(u*v*). The transformation of (a*, b*) to
(C*ab, hab) is given by:
The chroma in the CIE L*C*h(a*b*) and CIE L*C*h(u*v*) coordinates has the advantage of being more
psychovisually linear, yet they are non-linear in terms of linear component color mixing. And therefore, chroma in
CIE 1976 L*a*b* and L*u*v* color spaces is very much different from the traditional sense of "saturation".
References
[1] Mark D. Fairchild. “ Color Appearance Models: CIECAM02 and Beyond (http:/ / www. cis. rit. edu/ fairchild/ PDFs/ AppearanceLec. pdf)”.
Slides from a tutorial at the IS&T/SID 12th Color Imaging Conference. 9 November 2004. Retrieved 19 September 2007.
[2] Schanda, János (2007). Colorimetry: Understanding the CIE System (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=g8VDAgAACAAJ&
dq=intitle:Colorimetry+ intitle:Understanding+ intitle:the+ intitle:CIE+ intitle:System). Wiley Interscience. ISBN 978-0-470-04904-4. , page
88.
[3] Hunt, Robert William Gainer (1993). Leslie D. Stroebel, Richard D. Zakia. ed. The Focal Encyclopedia of Photography (http:/ / books.
google. com/ ?id=CU7-2ZLGFpYC& pg=PA124& dq="correlate+ of+ saturation"+ cielab+ chroma+ lightness+ chromaticity). Focal Press.
p. 124. ISBN 0-240-51417-3. .
[4] Lübbe, Eva (2010). Colours in the Mind - Colour Systems in Reality- A formula for colour saturation. [Book on Demand].
ISBN 978-3-7881-4057-1.
[5] Moroney, Nathan; Fairchild, Mark D.; Hunt, Robert W.G.; Li, Changjun; Luo, M. Ronnier; Newman, Todd (November 12 2002). "The
CIECAM02 Color Appearance Model" (http:/ / www. polybytes. com/ misc/ Meet_CIECAM02. pdf) (PDF). IS&T/SID Tenth Color Imaging
Conference. Scottsdale, Arizona: The Society for Imaging Science and Technology. ISBN 0-89208-241-0. .
[6] Juan, Lu-Yin G.; Luo, Ming R. (June 2002). "Magnitude estimation for scaling saturation" (http:/ / spiedl. aip. org/ getabs/ servlet/
GetabsServlet?prog=normal& id=PSISDG004421000001000575000001& idtype=cvips& gifs=yes). In Robert Chung, Allan Rodrigues.
Proceedings of SPIE. 4421. 9th Congress of the International Colour Association. pp. 575–578. doi:10.1117/12.464511. .
[7] Stroebel, Leslie D.; Zakia, Richard D. (1993). The Focal Encyclopedia of Photography (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=CU7-2ZLGFpYC&
pg=PA121& dq="excitation+ purity") (3E ed.). Focal Press. p. 121. ISBN 0-240-51417-3. .
Dichromatism 39
Dichromatism
Dichromatism (or polychromatism) is a phenomenon where a material or solution's hue is dependent on both the
concentration of the absorbing substance and the depth or thickness of the medium traversed.[1] In most substances
which are not dichromatic, only the brightness and saturation of the colour depend on their concentration and layer
thickness.
Examples of dichromatic substances are pumpkin seed oil, bromophenol blue and resazurin. When the layer of
pumpkin seed oil is less than 0.7 mm thick, the oil appears bright green, and in layer thicker than this, it appears
bright red.
The phenomenon is related to both the physical chemistry properties of the substance and the physiological response
of the human visual system to colour. This combined physicochemical–physiological basis was first explained in
2007.[2]
Physical explanation
Dichromatic properties can be explained by the Beer-Lambert law and by the excitation characteristics of the three
types of cone photoreceptors in the human retina. Dichromatism is potentially observable in any substance that has
an absorption spectrum with one wide but shallow local minimum and one narrow but deep local minimum. The
apparent width of the deep minimum may also be limited by the end of the visible range of human eye; in this case,
the true full width may not necessarily be narrow. As the thickness of the substance increases, the perceived hue
changes from that defined by the position of the wide-but-shallow minimum (in thin layers) to the hue of the
deep-but-narrow minimum (in thick layers).
The absorbance spectrum of pumpkin seed oil has the wide-but-shallow minimum in the green region of the
spectrum and deep local minimum in the red region. In thin layers, the absorption at any specific green wavelength is
not as low as it is for the red minimum, but a broader band of greenish wavelengths are transmitted, and hence the
overall appearance is green. The effect is enhanced by the greater sensitivity to green of the photoreceptors in the
human eye, and the narrowing of the red transmittance band by the long-wavelength limit of cone photoreceptor
sensitivity. According to the Beer-Lambert law, when viewing through the coloured substance (and thus ignoring
reflection), the proportion of light transmitted at a given wavelength, T, decreases exponentially with thickness t, T =
e-at, where a is the absorbance at that wavelength. Let Ge-aGt be the green transmittance and Re-aRt be the red
transmittance. The ratio of the two transmitted intensities is then (G/R)e(aR-aG)t. If the red absorbance is less than the
green, then as the thickness t increases, so does the ratio of red to green transmitted light, which causes the apparent
hue of the colour to switch from green to red.
Dichromatism 40
Quantification
The extent of dichromatism of material can be quantified by the Kreft's dichromaticity index (DI). It is defined as the
difference in hue angle (Δhab) between the colour of the sample at the dilution, where the chroma (colour saturation)
is maximal and the colour of four times more diluted (or thinner) and four times more concentrated (or thicker)
sample. The two hue angle differences are called dichromaticity index towards lighter (Kreft's DIL) and
dichromaticity index towards darker (Kreft's DID) respectively.[3] Kreft's dichromaticity index DIL and DID for
pumpkin oil, which is one of the most dichromatic substances, are −9 and −44, respectively. This means that
pumpkin oil changes its colour from green-yellow to orange-red (for 44 degrees in Lab colour space) when the
thickness of the observed layer is increased from cca 0.5 mm to 2 mm; and it changes slightly towards green (for 9
degrees) if its thickness is reduced for 4-fold.
History
A record by William Herschel (1738–1822), shows he observed dichromatism with a solution of ferrous sulphate in
1801 when working on an early solar telescope, but he did not recognise the effect.[4]
References
[1] Kennard IG, Howell DH (1941) Types of colouring in minerals. Am Mineral 26:405–421
[2] Kreft S and Kreft M (2007) Physicochemical and physiological basis of dichromatic colour, Naturwissenschaften 94, 935-939. On-line PDF
(http:/ / www. springerlink. com/ content/ h5630lr536pj1333/ fulltext. pdf)
[3] Kreft S, Kreft M. (2009). "Quantification of dichromatism: a characteristic of color in transparent materials" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase.
org/ josaa/ abstract. cfm?URI=josaa-26-7-1576). Journal of the Optical Society of America A 26 (7): 1576–1581.
Bibcode 2009JOSAA..26.1576K. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.26.001576. .
[4] The History of the Telescope - By Henry C. King - Page 141 (http:/ / books. google. com/ books?id=KAWwzHlDVksC& lpg=PA136&
dq=Lilienthal telescope& pg=PA141#v=onepage& q& f=false)
Hue 41
Hue
Hue is one of the main properties of a
color, defined technically (in the
CIECAM02 model), as "the degree to
which a stimulus can be described as
similar to or different from stimuli that
are described as red, green, blue, and Hue in the HSB/HSL encodings of RGB
Computing hue
In opponent color spaces in which two
of the axes are perceptually orthogonal
to lightness, such as the CIE 1976 (L*,
a*, b*) (CIELAB) and 1976 (L*, u*,
The hues in the image of this Painted
v*) (CIELUV) color spaces, hue may
Bunting are cyclically rotated over
be computed together with chroma by time.
converting these coordinates from
rectangular form to polar form. Hue is the angular component of the polar representation, while chroma is the radial
component.
Specifically, in CIELAB:[6]
Preucil used a polar plot, which he termed a color circle.[7] Using R, G, and B, one may compute hue angle using the
following scheme: determine which of the six possible orderings of R, G, and B prevail, then apply the formula
given in the table below.
Red-Yellow
Yellow-Green
Green-Cyan
Cyan-Blue
Blue-Magenta
Magenta-Red
Note that in each case the formula contains the fraction , where H is the highest of R, G, and B; L is the
lowest, and M is the mid one between the other two. This is referred to as the Preucil Hue Error, and was used in the
computation of mask strength in photomechanical color reproduction.[9]
Hue angles computed for the Preucil circle agree with the hue angle computed for the Preucil Hexagon at integer
multiples of 30 degrees (red, yellow, green, cyan, blue, magenta, and the colors mid-way between contiguous pairs)
and differ by approximately 1.2 degrees at odd integer multiples of 15 degrees (based on the circle formula), the
maximum divergence between the two.
Hue 43
The process of converting an RGB color into an HSL color space or HSV color space is usually based on a 6-piece
piecewise mapping, treating the HSV cone as a hexacone, or the HSL double cone as a double hexacone.[10] The
formulae used are those in the table above.
Specialized hues
The hues exhibited by caramel colorings and beers are fairly limited in range. The Linner hue index is used to
quantify the hue of such products.
Usage in art
Manufacturers of pigments use the word hue e.g. 'Cadmium Yellow (hue)' to indicate that the original pigmentation
ingredient, often toxic, has been replaced by safer (or cheaper) alternatives whilst retaining the hue of the original.
Replacements are often used for chromium, cadmium and alizarin.
Hue difference: or ?
There are two main ways in which hue difference is quantified. The first is the simple difference between the two
hue angles. The symbol for this expression of hue difference is in CIELAB and in CIELUV. The
other is computed as the residual total color difference after Lightness and Chroma differences have been accounted
for; its symbol is in CIELAB and in CIELUV.
References
[1] Mark Fairchild, "Color Appearance Models: CIECAM02 and Beyond." Tutorial slides for IS&T/SID 12th Color Imaging Conference.
[2] C J Bartleson, "Brown". Color Research and Application, 1 : 4, p 181-191 (1976).
[3] "The Color Wheel and Color Theory" (http:/ / creativecurio. com/ 2008/ 05/ the-color-wheel-and-color-theory/ ). Creative Curio. 2008-05-16.
. Retrieved 2011-06-09.
[4] Conway, BR; Moeller, S; Tsao, DY. (2007). "Specialized color modules in macaque extrastriate cortex". Neuron 56 (3): 560–73.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.008. PMID 17988638.
[5] Conway, BR; Tsao, DY (2009). "Color-tuned neurons are spatially clustered according to color preference within alert macaque posterior
inferior temporal cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 (42): 18034–9.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0810943106. PMC 2764907. PMID 19805195.
[6] Colorimetry, second edition: CIE Publication 15.2. Vienna: Bureau Central of the CIE, 1986.
[7] Frank Preucil, "Color Hue and Ink Transfer … Their Relation to Perfect Reproduction, TAGA Proceedings, p 102-110 (1953).
[8] Ralph Merrill Evans, W T Hanson, and W Lyle Brewer, Principles of Color Photography. New York: Wiley, 1953
[9] Miles Southworth, Color Separation Techniques, second edition. Livonia, New York: Graphic Arts Publishing, 1979
[10] Max K. Agoston (2004). Computer Graphics and Geometric Modelling v. 1: Implementation and Algorithms (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=fGX8yC-4vXUC& pg=PA301& lpg=PA301& dq=hsv+ + hue+ rgb#PPA304,M1). Springer. pp. 301–304. ISBN 1-85233-818-0. .
[11] Deane B Judd and Günter Wyszecki, Color in Business, Science, and Industry. New York: Wiley, 1976.
Hue 44
External links
• Editing of hue in photography (http://gimps.de/en/tutorials/gimp/picture-photo-image/improve-colors/)
It is common among some artistic painters to darken a paint color by adding black paint—producing colors called
shades—or to lighten a color by adding white—producing colors called tints. However, this is not always the best
way for representational painting, since an unfortunate result is for colors to also shift in their hues. For instance,
darkening a color by adding black can cause colors such as yellows, reds and oranges, to shift toward the greenish or
bluish part of the spectrum. Lightening a color by adding white can cause a shift towards blue when mixed with reds
and oranges. Another practice when darkening a color is to use its opposite, or complementary, color (e.g.
violet-purple added to yellowish-green) in order to neutralize it without a shift in hue, and darken it if the additive
color is darker than the parent color. When lightening a color this hue shift can be corrected with the addition of a
small amount of an adjacent color to bring the hue of the mixture back in line with the parent color (e.g. adding a
small amount of orange to a mixture of red and white will correct the tendency of this mixture to shift slightly
towards the blue end of the spectrum).
Tints and shades 45
An extension of the color wheel: the color sphere. Colors nearest the center or the poles are most achromatic. Colors of the same lightness and
saturation are of the same nuance. Colors of the same hue and saturation, but of different lightness, are said to be tints and shades. Colors of the same
hue and lightness, but of varying saturation, are called tones.
Lightness 46
Lightness
Lightness (sometimes called value or tone) is a property of a color, or a
dimension of a color space, that is defined in a way to reflect the
subjective brightness perception of a color for humans along a
lightness–darkness axis.
Various color models have an explicit term for this property. The
Munsell color model uses the term value, while the HSL color model
and Lab color space use the term lightness. The HSV model uses the
term value a little differently: a color with a low value is nearly black,
but one with a high value is the pure, fully saturated color.
In subtractive color (i.e. paints) value changes can be achieved by
adding black or white to the color. However, this also reduces
saturation. Chiaroscuro and Tenebrism both take advantage of dramatic
contrasts of value to heighten drama in art. Artists may also employ
shading, subtle manipulation of value.
Note: Munsell's V runs from 0 to 10, while Y typically runs from 0 to 100 (often interpreted as a percent). Typically,
the relative luminance is normalized so that the "reference white" (say, magnesium oxide) has a tristimulus value of
Y=100. Since the reflectance of magnesium oxide (MgO) relative to the perfect reflecting diffuser is 97.5%, V=10
corresponds to Y=100/97.5%≈102.6 if MgO is used as the reference.[3]
Lightness 47
1920
Priest et al. provide a basic
estimate of the Munsell value
(with Y running from 0 to 1 in
this case):[4]
1933
Munsell, Sloan, and Godlove
launch a study on the Munsell
neutral value scale, considering
several proposals relating the
relative luminance to the
Munsell value, and suggest:[5][6]
1943
Observe that the lightness is 50% for a luminance of around 18% relative to the reference
Newhall, Nickerson, and Judd white.
prepare a report for the Optical
Society of America. They suggest a quintic parabola (relating the reflectance in terms of the value):[7]
1943
Using Table II of the O.S.A. report, Moon and Spencer express the value in terms of the luminance:[8]
1944
Saunderson and Milner introduce a subtractive constant in the previous expression, for a better fit to the
Munsell value.[9] Later, Jameson and Hurvich claim that this corrects for simultaneous contrast effects.[10][11]
1955
Ladd and Pinney of Eastman Kodak are interested in the Munsell value as a perceptually uniform lightness
scale for use in television. After considering one logarithmic and five power-law functions (per Stevens' power
law), they relate value to reflectance by raising the reflectance to the power of 0.352:[12]
Realizing this is quite close to the cube root, they simplify it to:
1958
Glasser et al. define the lightness as ten times the Munsell value (so that the lightness ranges from 0 to
100):[13]
1964
Wyszecki simplifies this to:[14]
Lightness 48
This formula approximates the Munsell value function for (it is not applicable for Y<1%)
and is used for the CIE 1964 color space.
1976
CIELAB uses the following formula:
where is the Y tristimulus value of a "specified white object" and is subject to the restriction
. Pauli removes this restriction by computing a linear extrapolation which maps Y/Yn=0 to
*
L =0 and is tangent to the formula above at the point at which the linear extension takes effect. First, the
transition point is determined to be , then the slope of
is computed. This gives the two-part function:[15]
An approximately 18% grey card, having an exact reflectance of , has a lightness value of 50. It is called
"mid grey" because its lightness is midway between black and white.
References
[1] Kuehni, Rolf G. (February 2002). "The early development of the Munsell system". Color Research & Application 27 (1): 20–27.
doi:10.1002/col.10002.
[2] Hunt, Robert W. G. (May 18 1957). "Light Energy and Brightness Sensation". Nature 179 (4568): 1026. doi:10.1038/1791026a0.
[3] Valberg, Arne (2006). Light Vision Color (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=hNDS1C6x0WYC& pg=PA200& dq=0. 975+ OR+ 97. 5+
"magnesium+ oxide"). John Wiley and Sons. p. 200. ISBN 0470849029. .
[4] Priest, Irwin G.; Gibson, K.S.; McNicholas, H.J. (September 1920). An examination of the Munsell color system. I: Spectral and total
reflection and the Munsell scale of Value. Technical paper 167. United States Bureau of Standards. p. 27
[5] Munsell, A.E.O.; Sloan, L.L.; Godlove, I.H. (November 1933). "Neutral value scales. I. Munsell neutral value scale" (http:/ / www.
opticsinfobase. org/ abstract. cfm?URI=josa-23-11-394). JOSA 23 (11): 394–411. doi:10.1364/JOSA.23.000394. . Note: This paper contains a
historical survey stretching to 1760.
[6] Munsell, A.E.O.; Sloan, L.L.; Godlove, I.H. (December 1933). "Neutral value scales. II. A comparison of results and equations describing
value scales" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase. org/ abstract. cfm?URI=josa-23-12-419). JOSA 23 (12): 419–425. doi:10.1364/JOSA.23.000419. .
[7] Newhall, Sidney M.; Nickerson, Dorothy; Judd, Deane B (May 1943). "Final report of the O.S.A. subcommittee on the spacing of the
Munsell colors" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase. org/ abstract. cfm?URI=josa-33-7-385). Journal of the Optical Society of America 33 (7):
385–418. doi:10.1364/JOSA.33.000385. .
[8] Moon, Parry; Spencer, Domina Eberle (May 1943). "Metric based on the composite color stimulus" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase. org/
abstract. cfm?URI=josa-33-5-270). JOSA 33 (5): 270–277. doi:10.1364/JOSA.33.000270. .
[9] Saunderson, Jason L.; Milner, B.I. (March 1944). "Further study of ω space" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase. org/ abstract.
cfm?URI=josa-34-3-167). JOSA 34 (3): 167–173. doi:10.1364/JOSA.34.000167. .
[10] Hurvich, Leo M.; Jameson, Dorothea (November 1957). "An Opponent-Process Theory of Color Vision" (http:/ / psycnet. apa. org/ index.
cfm?fa=buy. optionToBuy& id=1959-02846-001). Psychological Review 64 (6): 384–404. doi:10.1037/h0041403. PMID 13505974. .
[11] Jameson, Dorothea; Leo M., Hurvich (May 1964). "Theory of brightness and color contrast in human vision". Vision Research 4 (1-2):
135–154. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(64)90037-9. PMID 5888593.
[12] Ladd, J.H.; Pinney, J.E. (September 1955). "Empirical relationships with the Munsell Value scale" (http:/ / ieeexplore. ieee. org/ xpls/
abs_all. jsp?isnumber=4055542& arnumber=4055558). Proceedings of the Institute of Radio Engineers 43 (9): 1137.
doi:10.1109/JRPROC.1955.277892. .
[13] Glasser, L.G.; A.H. McKinney, C.D. Reilly, and P.D. Schnelle (October 1958). "Cube-root color coordinate system" (http:/ / www.
opticsinfobase. org/ abstract. cfm?URI=josa-48-10-736). JOSA 48 (10): 736–740. doi:10.1364/JOSA.48.000736. .
[14] Wyszecki, Günther (November 1963). "Proposal for a New Color-Difference Formula" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase. org/ abstract.
cfm?URI=josa-53-11-1314). JOSA 53 (11): 1318–1319. doi:10.1364/JOSA.53.001318. . Note: The asterisks are not used in the paper.
Lightness 49
[15] Pauli, Hartmut K.A. (1976). "Proposed extension of the CIE recommendation on "Uniform color spaces, color spaces, and color-difference
equations, and metric color terms"" (http:/ / www. opticsinfobase. org/ abstract. cfm?URI=josa-66-8-866). JOSA 66 (8): 866–867.
doi:10.1364/JOSA.66.000866. .
50
Perception of Color
Opponent process
The color opponent process is a color theory that states
that the human visual system interprets information about
color by processing signals from cones and rods in an
antagonistic manner. The three types of cones (L for long,
M for medium and S for short) have some overlap in the
wavelengths of light to which they respond, so it is more
efficient for the visual system to record differences
between the responses of cones, rather than each type of
cone's individual response. The opponent color theory
suggests that there are three opponent channels: red
versus green, blue versus yellow, and black versus white
Opponent colors based on experiment. Deuteranopes see little
(the last type is achromatic and detects light-dark
difference between the two colors in the central column.
variation, or luminance).[1] Responses to one color of an
opponent channel are antagonistic to those to the other
color. That is, opposite opponent colors are never perceived together – there is no "greenish red" or "yellowish blue".
While the trichromatic theory defines the way the retina of the eye allows the visual system to detect color with three
types of cones, the opponent process theory accounts for mechanisms that receive and process information from
cones. Though the trichromatic and opponent processes theories were initially thought to be at odds, it later came to
be understood that the mechanisms responsible for the opponent process receive signals from the three types of
cones and process them at a more complex level.[2]
Besides the cones, which detect light entering the eye, the biological basis of the opponent theory involves two other
types of cells: bipolar cells, and ganglion cells. Information from the cones is passed to the bipolar cells in the retina,
which may be the cells in the opponent process that transform the information from cones. The information is then
passed to ganglion cells, of which there are two major classes: magnocellular, or large-cell layers, and parvocellular,
or small-cell layers. Parvocellular cells, or P cells, handle the majority of information about color, and fall into two
groups: one that processes information about differences between firing of L and M cones, and one that processes
differences between S cones and a combined signal from both L and M cones. The first subtype of cells are
responsible for processing red–green differences, and the second process blue–yellow differences. P cells also
transmit information about intensity of light (how much of it there is) due to their receptive fields.
Opponent process 51
History
Johann Wolfgang von Goethe first studied the physiological effect of opposed colors in his Theory of Colours in
1810.[3] Goethe arranged his color wheel symmetrically, "for the colours diametrically opposed to each other in this
diagram are those which reciprocally evoke each other in the eye. Thus, yellow demands purple; orange, blue; red,
green; and vice versa: thus again all intermediate gradations reciprocally evoke each other."[4][5]
Ewald Hering proposed opponent color theory in 1892.[6] He thought that the colors red, yellow, green, and blue are
special in that any other color can be described as a mix of them, and that they exist in opposite pairs. That is, either
red or green is perceived and never greenish-red; although yellow is a mixture of red and green in the RGB color
theory, the eye does not perceive it as such.
In 1957, Hurvich and Dorothea Jameson provided quantitative data for Hering's color opponency theory. Their
method was called "hue cancellation". Hue cancellation experiments start with a color (e.g. yellow) and attempt to
determine how much of the opponent color (e.g. blue) of one of the starting color's components must be added to
eliminate any hint of that component from the starting color (Wolfe, Kluender, & Levi, 2009).[7]
Griggs expanded the concept to reflect a wide range of opponent processes for biological systems in this book
Biological Relativity (c) 1967.
In 1970, Richard Solomon expanded Hurvich's general neurological opponent process model to explain emotion,
drug addiction, and work motivation. (See Opponent-process theory.)[8][9]
The opponent color theory can be applied to computer vision and implemented as the "Gaussian color model."[10]
Complementary-color afterimages
If we stare at a red square for forty seconds, and immediately look at a
white sheet of paper we'll often perceive a green square on the blank
sheet. This complementary color afterimage is more easily explained
by the opponent theory than the trichromatic; in the opponent-process
theory, fatigue of pathways promoting red produce the illusion of a
green square.[11]
Reddish green and yellowish blue A snapshot of the "color dove illusion"
"[s]ome observers indicated that although they were aware that what they were viewing was a color (that is,
the field was not achromatic), they were unable to name or describe the color. One of these observers was an
artist with a large color vocabulary. Other observers of the novel hues described the first stimulus as a
reddish-green."[13]
However, some subjects in the Crane and Piantanida study merely reported seeing hallucinatory textures, such as
blue specks on a yellow backdrop. A possible explanation is that the study did not control for variations in the
Opponent process 52
perceived luminance of the colors from subject to subject (two colors are equiluminant for an observer when rapidly
alternating between the colors produces the least impression of flickering). To investigate this, Vincent Billock,
Gerald Gleason and Brian Tsou set up a similar experiment which controlled for luminance.[14] They had the
following observation:
"We found that when colors were equiluminant, subjects saw reddish greens, bluish yellows, or a multistable
spatial color exchange (an entirely novel perceptual phenomena [sic]); when the colors were
nonequiluminant, subjects saw spurious pattern formation."
This led them to propose a 'soft-wired model of cortical color opponency', in which populations of neurons compete
to fire and in which the 'losing' neurons go completely silent. In this model, eliminating competition by, for instance,
inhibiting connections between neural populations can allow mutually exclusive neurons to fire together.[14]
References
[1] Michael Foster (1891). A Text-book of physiology (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=Swn8ztLFTdkC& pg=RA1-PA921& dq=hering+
red-green+ yellow-blue+ young-helmholtz+ date:0-1923). Lea Bros. & Co. p. 921. .
[2] Kandel ER, Schwartz JH and Jessell TM, 2000. Principles of Neural Science, 4th ed., McGraw–Hill, New York. pp. 577–80.
[3] "Goethe's Color Theory" (http:/ / webexhibits. org/ colorart/ ch. html). Vision science and the emergence of modern art. .
[4] Goethe, Johann (1810). Theory of Colours, paragraph #50.
[5] "Goethe on Colours" (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=H7TlAAAAMAAJ& pg=RA1-PA121& dq="reciprocally+ evoke+ each+ other+ in+
the+ eye"#v=onepage& q="reciprocally evoke each other in the eye"& f=false). The Art-Union 2 (18): 107. July 15, 1840. .
[6] Hering E, 1964. Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.
[7] Hurvich, Leo M.; Jameson, Dorothea (November 1957). "An opponent-process theory of color vision". Psychological Review 64 (6, Part I):
384–404. doi:10.1037/h0041403. PMID 13505974.
[8] Solomon, R.L. and Corbit, J.D. (1973). "An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation: II. Cigarette Addiction". Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 81 (2), pp. 158–171.
[9] Solomon, R.L. and Corbit, J.D. (1974). "An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation: I. Temporal Dynamics of Affect". Psychological
Review 81 (2), pp. 119–145.
[10] Geusebroek, J.-M.; van den Boomgaard, R.; Smeulders, A.W.M.; Geerts, H. (December 2001). "Color invariance". Pattern Analysis and
Machine Intelligence, IEEE Transactions on 23 (12): 1338–1350. doi:10.1109/34.977559.
[11] Griggs, R. A. (2009). "SENSATION AND PERCEPTION". Psychology: A Concise Introduction (2 ed.). Worth Publishers. p. 92.
ISBN 978-1-4292-0082-0. OCLC 213815202. "color information is processed at the post-receptor cell level (by bipolar, ganglion, thalamic,
and cortical cells) according to the opponent-process theory."
[12] *Crane HD and Piantanida TP, 1983. On Seeing Reddish Green and Yellowish Blue. Science, 221:1078–80.
[13] Suarez J; Suarez, Juan (2009). "Reddish Green: A Challenge for Modal Claims About Phenomenal Structure". Philosophy and
Phenomenological Research 78 (2): 346. doi:10.1111/j.1933-1592.2009.00247.x.
[14] Billock, Vincent A.; Gerald A. Gleason, Brian H. Tsou (2001). "Perception of forbidden colors in retinally stabilized equiluminant images:
an indication of softwired cortical color opponency?" (http:/ / aris. ss. uci. edu/ ~kjameson/ BillockEtAlImpossibleColorsJOSA2001. pdf).
Journal of the Optical Society of America A (Optical Society of America) 18 (10): 2398–2403. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.18.002398. . Retrieved
2010-08-21.
Further reading
• Baccus SA, 2007. Timing and computation in inner retinal circuitry. Annu Rev Physiol, 69:271–90.
• Masland RH, 2001. Neuronal diversity in the retina. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 11(4):431–6.
• Masland RH, 2001. The fundamental plan of the retina. Nat Neurosci. 4(9):877–86.
• Sowden PT and Schyns PG, 2006. Channel surfing in the visual brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 10(12):538–45.
• Wässle H, 2004. Parallel processing in the mammalian retina. Nat Rev Neurosci, 5(10):747–57.
Impossible colors 53
Impossible colors
Reddish Green redirects here. Or see Reddish (an area of the Metropolitan Borough of Stockport, in Greater
Manchester, England).
Impossible colors or forbidden colors are hues that cannot be perceived in ordinary viewing conditions from light
that is a combination of various intensities of the various frequencies of visible light. Examples of impossible colors
are bluish-yellow and reddish-green.[1] This does not mean the muddy brown color created when mixing red and
green paints, or the green color from mixing yellow and blue paints, but rather colors that appear to be similar to, for
example, both red and green, or both yellow and blue. Other colors never experienced in ordinary viewing, but
perceivable under special artificial laboratory conditions, would also be termed impossible colors.
Opponent process
The color opponent process is a color theory
that states that the human visual system
interprets information about color by
processing signals from cones and rods in an
antagonistic manner. The three types of
cones have some overlap in the wavelengths
of light to which they respond, so it is more
efficient for the visual system to record
differences between the responses of cones,
rather than each type of cone's individual
response. The opponent color theory Where opposing colors cancel each other out, the remaining color on the vertical
suggests that there are three opponent axis is perceived. However, under special conditions, a mixture of opposing colors
can be seen without the remaining color interfering.
channels: red versus green, blue versus
yellow, and black versus white (the latter
type is achromatic and detects light-dark variation, or luminance). Responses to one color of an opponent channel are
antagonistic to those to the other color.
Other researchers dispute the existence of colors forbidden by opponency theory and claim they are, in reality,
intermediate colors.[2] See also binocular rivalry.
Impossible colors 54
References
[1] Crane, Hewitt D.; Piantanida, Thomas P. (1983). "On Seeing Reddish Green and Yellowish Blue". Science 221 (4615): 1078–80.
doi:10.1126/science.221.4615.1078. JSTOR 1691544. PMID 17736657.
[2] Hsieh, P.-J.; Tse, P.U. (2006). "Illusory color mixing upon perceptual fading and filling-in does not result in 'forbidden colors'". Vision
Research 46 (14): 2251–8. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.11.030. PMID 16469353.
Further reading
• Billock, Vincent A.; Tsou, Brian H. (2010). "Seeing Forbidden Colors". Scientific American 302 (2): 72–7.
doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0210-72. PMID 20128226.
• Takahashi, Shigeko; Ejima, Yoshimichi (1984). "Spatial properties of red-green and yellow-blue perceptual
opponent-color response". Vision Research 24 (9): 987–94. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(84)90075-0. PMID 6506487.
• Hibino, H (1992). "Red-green and yellow-blue opponent-color responses as a function of retinal eccentricity".
Vision research 32 (10): 1955–64. PMID 1287992.
Color vision
Color vision is the ability of an organism or machine to distinguish
objects based on the wavelengths (or frequencies) of the light they
reflect, emit, or transmit. Colors can be measured and quantified in
various ways; indeed, a human's perception of colors is a subjective
process whereby the brain responds to the stimuli that are produced
when incoming light reacts with the several types of cone
photoreceptors in the eye. In essence different people may see one
subject in different ways.
Isaac Newton discovered that white light splits into its component
colors when passed through a dispersive prism, but that if those bands of colored light pass through another and
rejoin, they make a white beam. The characteristic colors are, from low to high frequency: red, orange, yellow,
green, cyan, blue, violet. Sufficient differences in frequency give rise to a difference in perceived hue; the just
noticeable difference in wavelength varies from about 1 nm in the blue-green and yellow wavelengths, to 10 nm and
more in the red and blue. Though the eye can distinguish up to a few hundred hues, when those pure spectral colors
are mixed together or diluted with white light, the number of distinguishable chromaticities can be quite high.
In very low light levels, vision is scotopic: light is detected by rod cells of the retina. Rods are maximally sensitive to
wavelengths near 500 nm, and play little, if any, role in color vision. In brighter light, such as daylight, vision is
photopic: light is detected by cone cells which are responsible for color vision. Cones are sensitive to a range of
wavelengths, but are most sensitive to wavelengths near 555 nm. Between these regions, mesopic vision comes into
play and both rods and cones provide signals to the retinal ganglion cells. The shift in color perception from dim
light to daylight gives rise to differences known as the Purkinje effect.
The perception of "white" is formed by the entire spectrum of visible light, or by mixing colors of just a few
wavelengths, such as red, green, and blue, or by mixing just a pair of complementary colors such as blue and
yellow.[1]
Color vision 55
For example, while the L cones have been referred to simply as red
receptors, microspectrophotometry has shown that their peak
sensitivity is in the greenish-yellow region of the spectrum. Similarly,
the S- and M-cones do not directly correspond to blue and green,
Normalized response spectra of human cones, S,
although they are often depicted as such. It is important to note that the M, and L types, to monochromatic spectral
RGB color model is merely a convenient means for representing color, stimuli, with wavelength given in nanometers.
and is not directly based on the types of cones in the human eye.
The peak response of human cone cells varies, even among individuals
with 'normal' color vision;[3] in some non-human species this
polymorphic variation is even greater, and it may well be adaptive.[4]
antagonistic way: red vs. green, blue vs. yellow, black vs. white. We
now know both theories to be correct, describing different stages in visual physiology, visualized in the diagram on
the right.[6] Green ←→ Magenta and Blue ←→ Yellow are scales with mutually exclusive boundaries. In the same
way that there cannot exist a "slightly negative" positive number, one cannot perceive a blueish-yellow or a
reddish-green.
Color vision 56
S β 400–500 nm 420–440 nm
M γ 450–630 nm 534–555 nm
L ρ 500–700 nm 564–580 nm
A range of wavelengths of light stimulates each of these receptor types to varying degrees. Yellowish-green light, for
example, stimulates both L and M cones equally strongly, but only stimulates S-cones weakly. Red light, on the
other hand, stimulates L cones much more than M cones, and S cones hardly at all; blue-green light stimulates M
cones more than L cones, and S cones a bit more strongly, and is also the peak stimulant for rod cells; and blue light
stimulates S cones more strongly than red or green light, but L and M cones more weakly. The brain combines the
information from each type of receptor to give rise to different perceptions of different wavelengths of light.
The opsins (photopigments) present in the L and M cones are encoded on the X chromosome; defective encoding of
these leads to the two most common forms of color blindness. The OPN1LW gene, which codes for the opsin present
in the L cones, is highly polymorphic (a recent study by Verrelli and Tishkoff found 85 variants in a sample of 236
men).[9] A very small percentage of women may have an extra type of color receptor because they have different
alleles for the gene for the L opsin on each X chromosome. X chromosome inactivation means that only one opsin is
expressed in each cone cell, and some women may therefore show a degree of tetrachromatic color vision.[10]
Variations in OPN1MW, which codes the opsin expressed in M cones, appear to be rare, and the observed variants
have no effect on spectral sensitivity.
Color vision 57
Visual information is then sent to the brain from retinal ganglion cells via the optic nerve to the optic chiasma: a
point where the two optic nerves meet and information from the temporal (contralateral) visual field crosses to the
other side of the brain. After the optic chiasma the visual tracts are referred to as the optic tracts, which enter the
thalamus to synapse at the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN).
The lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) is divided into laminae (zones), of which there are three types: the M-laminae,
consisting primarily of M-cells, the P-laminae, consisting primarily of P-cells, and the koniocellular laminae. M- and
P-cells received relatively balanced input from both L- and M-cones throughout most of the retina, although this
seems to not be the case at the fovea, with midget cells synapsing in the P-laminae. The koniocellular laminae
receive axons from the small bistratified ganglion cells.[11][12]
After synapsing at the LGN, the visual tract continues on back to the primary visual cortex (V1) located at the back
of the brain within the occipital lobe. Within V1 there is a distinct band (striation). This is also referred to as "striate
cortex", with other cortical visual regions referred to collectively as "extrastriate cortex". It is at this stage that color
processing becomes much more complicated.
In V1 the simple three-color segregation begins to break down. Many cells in V1 respond to some parts of the
spectrum better than others, but this "color tuning" is often different depending on the adaptation state of the visual
system. A given cell that might respond best to long wavelength light if the light is relatively bright might then
become responsive to all wavelengths if the stimulus is relatively dim. Because the color tuning of these cells is not
stable, some believe that a different, relatively small, population of neurons in V1 is responsible for color vision.
These specialized "color cells" often have receptive fields that can compute local cone ratios. Such
"double-opponent" cells were initially described in the goldfish retina by Nigel Daw;[13][14] their existence in
primates was suggested by David H. Hubel and Torsten Wiesel and subsequently proven by Bevil Conway.[15] As
Margaret Livingstone and David Hubel showed, double opponent cells are clustered within localized regions of V1
called blobs, and are thought to come in two flavors, red–green and blue–yellow.[16] Red–green cells compare the
relative amounts of red–green in one part of a scene with the amount of red–green in an adjacent part of the scene,
responding best to local color contrast (red next to green). Modeling studies have shown that double-opponent cells
are ideal candidates for the neural machinery of color constancy explained by Edwin H. Land in his retinex
theory.[17]
Color vision 58
Anatomical studies have shown that neurons in extended V4 provide input to the inferior temporal lobe . "IT" cortex
is thought to integrate color information with shape and form, although it has been difficult to define the appropriate
criteria for this claim. Despite this murkiness, it has been useful to characterize this pathway (V1 > V2 > V4 > IT) as
the ventral stream or the "what pathway", distinguished from the dorsal stream ("where pathway") that is thought to
analyze motion, among many other features.
shades (Zuzu in Himba), very light (Vapa), Vivid blue and green
(Buru) and dry colors as an adaptation to their specific way of life.
Perception of color depends heavily on the context in which the perceived object is presented. For example, a white
page under blue, pink, or purple light will reflect mostly blue, pink, or purple light to the eye, respectively; the brain,
however, compensates for the effect of lighting (based on the color shift of surrounding objects) and is more likely to
interpret the page as white under all three conditions, a phenomenon known as color constancy.
Color vision 59
Evolution
Color perception mechanisms are highly dependent on evolutionary factors, of which the most prominent is thought
to be satisfactory recognition of food sources. In herbivorous primates, color perception is essential for finding
proper (immature) leaves. In hummingbirds, particular flower types are often recognized by color as well. On the
other hand, nocturnal mammals have less-developed color vision, since adequate light is needed for cones to
function properly. There is evidence that ultraviolet light plays a part in color perception in many branches of the
animal kingdom, especially insects. In general, the optical spectrum encompasses the most common electronic
transitions in matter and is therefore the most useful for collecting information about the environment.
The evolution of trichromatic color vision in primates occurred as the ancestors of modern monkeys, apes, and
humans switched to diurnal (daytime) activity and began consuming fruits and leaves from flowering plants.[37]
Color vision, with UV discrimination, is also present in a number of arthropods – the only terrestrial animals besides
the vertebrates to possess this trait.[38]
Some animals can distinguish colors in the ultraviolet spectrum. The UV spectrum falls outside the human visible
range, except for some cataract surgery patients.[39] Birds, turtles, lizards, many fish and some rodents have UV
receptors in their retinas.[40] These animals can see the UV patterns found on flowers and other wildlife that are
otherwise invisible to the human eye.
UV and multi-dimensional vision is an especially important adaptation in birds. It allows birds to spot small prey
from a distance, navigate, avoid predators, and forage while flying at high speeds. Birds also utilize their broad
spectrum vision to recognize other birds, and in sexual selection.[41][42]
Technically, the image of the (mathematical) cone over the simplex whose vertices are the spectral colors, by this
linear mapping, is also a (mathematical) cone in R3color. Moving directly away from the vertex of this cone
represents maintaining the same chromaticity while increasing its intensity. Taking a cross-section of this cone yields
a 2D chromaticity space. Both the 3D cone and its projection or cross-section are convex sets; that is, any mixture of
spectral colors is also a color.
In practice, it would be quite difficult to physiologically measure an
individual's three cone responses to various physical color stimuli.
Instead, a psychophysical approach is taken. Three specific benchmark
test lights are typically used; let us call them S, M, and L. To calibrate
human perceptual space, scientists allowed human subjects to try to
match any physical color by turning dials to create specific
combinations of intensities (IS, IM, IL) for the S, M, and L lights, resp.,
until a match was found. This needed only to be done for physical
colors that are spectral (since a linear combination of spectral colors
will be matched by the same linear combination of their (IS, IM, IL)
matches. Note that in practice, often at least one of S, M, L would have
to be added with some intensity to the physical test color, and that
combination matched by a linear combination of the remaining 2
The CIE 1931 xy chromaticity diagram. The
lights. Across different individuals (without color blindness), the
Planckian locus is shown with color temperatures
matchings turned out to be nearly identical. labeled in degrees Kelvin. The outer curved
boundary is the spectral (or monochromatic)
By considering all the resulting combinations of intensities (IS, IM, IL) locus, with wavelengths shown in nanometers
as a subset of 3-space, a model for human perceptual color space is (blue). Note that the colors in this file are being
formed. (Note that when one of S, M, L had to be added to the test specified using sRGB. Areas outside the triangle
cannot be accurately rendered because they are
color, its intensity was counted as negative.) Again, this turns out to be
out of the gamut of sRGB, therefore they have
a (mathematical) cone, not a quadric, but rather all rays through the been interpreted. Note that the colors depicted
origin in 3-space passing through a certain convex set. Again, this cone depend on the color space of the device you use
has the property that moving directly away from the origin corresponds to view the image (number of colors on your
monitor, etc.), and may not be a strictly accurate
to increasing the intensity of the S, M, L lights proportionately. Again,
representation of the color at a particular position.
a cross-section of this cone is a planar shape that is (by definition) the
space of "chromaticities" (informally: distinct colors); one particular
such cross section, corresponding to constant X+Y+Z of the CIE 1931 color space, gives the CIE chromaticity
diagram.
It should be noted that this system implies that for any hue or non-spectral color not on the boundary of the
chromaticity diagram, there are infinitely many distinct physical spectra that are all perceived as that hue or color.
So, in general there is no such thing as the combination of spectral colors that we perceive as (say) a specific version
of tan; instead there are infinitely many possibilities that produce that exact color. The boundary colors that are pure
spectral colors can be perceived only in response to light that is purely at the associated wavelength, while the
boundary colors on the "line of purples" can each only be generated by a specific ratio of the pure violet and the pure
red at the ends of the visible spectral colors.
The CIE chromaticity diagram is horseshoe-shaped, with its curved edge corresponding to all spectral colors (the
spectral locus), and the remaining straight edge corresponding to the most saturated purples, mixtures of red and
violet.
Color vision 62
Chromatic adaptation
In color science, chromatic adaptation is the estimation of the representation of an object under a different light
source than the one in which it was recorded. A common application is to find a chromatic adaptation transform
(CAT) that will make the recording of a neutral object appear neutral (color balance), while keeping other colors also
looking realistic.[43] For example, chromatic adaptation transforms are used when converting images between ICC
profiles with different white points. Adobe Photoshop, for example, uses the Bradford CAT.[44]
In color vision, chromatic adaptation refers to color constancy; the ability of the visual system to preserve the
appearance of an object under a wide range of light sources.[45]
References
[1] "Eye, human." Encyclopædia Britannica 2006 Ultimate Reference Suite DVD, 2009.
[2] Nathans; Thomas, Darcy; Hogness, David S.. "Molecular Genetics of Human Color Vision: The Genes Encoding Blue, Green, and Red
Pigments" (http:/ / www. jstor. org. myaccess. library. utoronto. ca/ stable/ 169687). Science, New Series, Vol. 232, No. 4747 (Apr. 11, 1986),
pp. 193-202. American Association for the Advancement of Science. .
[3] Neitz J, Jacobs GH (1986). "Polymorphism of the long-wavelength cone in normal human color vision" (http:/ / www. nature. com/ nature/
journal/ v323/ n6089/ abs/ 323623a0. html). Nature 323 (6089): 623–5. doi:10.1038/323623a0. PMID 3773989. .
[4] Jacobs GH (January 1996). "Primate photopigments and primate color vision". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93 (2): 577–81.
doi:10.1073/pnas.93.2.577. PMC 40094. PMID 8570598.
[5] Hering, Ewald (1872). "Zur Lehre vom Lichtsinne" (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=u5MCAAAAYAAJ& pg=PA5& lpg=PA5& dq=1872+
hering+ ewald+ Zur+ Lehre+ vom+ Lichtsinne. + Sitzungsberichte+ der+ kaiserlichen+ Akademie+ der+ Wissenschaften. +
Mathematischânaturwissenschaftliche+ Classe,). Sitzungsberichte der Mathematisch–Naturwissenschaftliche Classe der Kaiserlichen
Akademie der Wissenschaften (K.-K. Hof- und Staatsdruckerei in Commission bei C. Gerold's Sohn) LXVI. Band (III Abtheilung). .
[6] Ali, M.A. & Klyne, M.A. (1985), p.168
[7] Wyszecki, Günther; Stiles, W.S. (1982). Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae (2nd ed.). New York:
Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics. ISBN 0-471-02106-7.
[8] R. W. G. Hunt (2004). The Reproduction of Colour (6th ed.). Chichester UK: Wiley–IS&T Series in Imaging Science and Technology.
pp. 11–2. ISBN 0-470-02425-9.
[9] Verrelli BC, Tishkoff SA (September 2004). "Signatures of Selection and Gene Conversion Associated with Human Color Vision Variation".
Am. J. Hum. Genet. 75 (3): 363–75. doi:10.1086/423287. PMC 1182016. PMID 15252758.
[10] Roth, Mark (2006). "Some women may see 100 million colors, thanks to their genes" (http:/ / www. post-gazette. com/ pg/ 06256/
721190-114. stm) Post-Gazette.com
[11] R.W. Rodieck, "The First Steps in Seeing". Sinauer Associates, Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts, USA, 1998.
[12] "SH Hendry, RC Reid, "The Koniocellular Pathway in Primate Vision". Annual Reviews Neuroscience, 2000, vol. 23, pp. 127-53" (http:/ /
www. annualreviews. org/ doi/ abs/ 10. 1146/ annurev. neuro. 23. 1. 127?url_ver=Z39. 88-2003& rfr_id=ori:rid:crossref. org&
rfr_dat=cr_pub=ncbi. nlm. nih. gov). Annualreviews.org. 1970-01-01. . Retrieved 2012-09-09.
[13] Nigel W. Daw (17 November 1967). "Goldfish Retina: Organization for Simultaneous Color Contrast". Science 158 (3803): 942–4.
doi:10.1126/science.158.3803.942. PMID 6054169.
[14] Bevil R. Conway (2002). Neural Mechanisms of Color Vision: Double-Opponent Cells in the Visual Cortex (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=pFodUlHfQmcC& pg=PR7& dq=goldfish+ retina+ by+ Nigel-Daw). Springer. ISBN 1-4020-7092-6. .
[15] Conway BR (15 April 2001). "Spatial structure of cone inputs to color cells in alert macaque primary visual cortex (V-1)" (http:/ / www.
jneurosci. org/ content/ 21/ 8/ 2768. full). J. Neurosci. 21 (8): 2768–83. PMID 11306629. .
[16] John E. Dowling (2001). Neurons and Networks: An Introduction to Behavioral Neuroscience (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=adeUwgfwdKwC& pg=PA376& dq=Margaret+ Livingstone+ David+ Hubel+ double+ opponent+ blobs). Harvard University Press.
ISBN 0-674-00462-0. .
[17] McCann, M., ed. 1993. Edwin H. Land's Essays. Springfield, Va.: Society for Imaging Science and Technology.
[18] Judd, Deane B.; Wyszecki, Günter (1975). Color in Business, Science and Industry. Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics (3rd ed.). New
York: Wiley-Interscience. p. 388. ISBN 0-471-45212-2.
[19] Conway BR, Moeller S, Tsao DY (2007). "Specialized color modules in macaque extrastriate cortex". Neuron 56 (3): 560–73.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.008. PMID 17988638.
[20] Conway BR, Tsao DY (2009). "Color-tuned neurons are spatially clustered according to color preference within alert macaque posterior
inferior temporal cortex". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106 (42): 18035–18039. doi:10.1073/pnas.0810943106. PMC 2764907. PMID 19805195.
[21] John Allman and Steven W. Zucker (1993). "On cytochrome oxidase blobs in visual cortex" (http:/ / books. google. com/
?id=eWBiKaOCNIYC& pg=PA34& dq=v4+ zeki+ color). In Laurence Harris and Michael Jenkin, editors. Spatial Vision in Humans and
Robots: The Proceedings of the 1991 York Conference. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-43071-2. .
[22] Roberson, Davidoff, Davies & Shapiro. referred by Debi Roberson, University of Essex 2011
Color vision 63
[23] JACOBS, GERALD H. (August 1993). "THE DISTRIBUTION AND NATURE OF COLOUR VISION AMONG THE MAMMALS"
(http:/ / onlinelibrary. wiley. com/ doi/ 10. 1111/ j. 1469-185X. 1993. tb00738. x/ abstract). Biological Reviews Volume 68, Issue 3, pages
413–471. . doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1993.tb00738.x
[24] Osorio D, Vorobyev M (June 2008). "A review of the evolution of animal colour vision and visual communication signals". Vision Research
48: 2042–2051. doi:10.1016/j.visres.
[25] Arikawa K (November 2003). "Spectral organization of the eye of a butterfly, Papilio" (http:/ / www. springerlink. com/ content/
whjepqnhpulyeevk/ ). J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 189 (11): 791–800. doi:10.1007/s00359-003-0454-7.
PMID 14520495. .
[26] Cronin TW, Marshall NJ (1989). "A retina with at least ten spectral types of photoreceptors in a mantis shrimp" (http:/ / www. nature. com/
nature/ journal/ v339/ n6220/ abs/ 339137a0. html). Nature 339 (6220): 137–40. doi:10.1038/339137a0. .
[27] Kelber A, Vorobyev M, Osorio D (February 2003). "Animal color vision—behavioural tests and physiological concepts". Biol Rev Camb
Philos Soc 78 (1): 81–118. doi:10.1017/S1464793102005985. PMID 12620062.
[28] Introducing Comparative Colour Vision (http:/ / books. google. com/ books?id=fB0madWbjBIC& pg=PA149& lpg=PA149&
dq=pentachromatic+ pigeons& source=bl& ots=205cOSKsEn& sig=aS3Xcqqf6FY_Yf1mHsbtVhgIHxY& hl=en& sa=X&
ei=2BD4T4OWC8XV6wH2p6XcBg& ved=0CGEQ6AEwBA#v=onepage& q=pentachromatic pigeons& f=false) Colour Vision: A Study in
Cognitive Science and the Philosophy of Perception, By Evan Thompson
[29] Roth, Lina S. V.; Lundström, Linda; Kelber, Almut; Kröger, Ronald H. H.; Unsbo, Peter (March 30, 2009). "The pupils and optical systems
of gecko eyes" (http:/ / www. journalofvision. org/ content/ 9/ 3/ 27). Journal of Vision 9 (3:27): 1–11. doi:10.1167/9.3.27. PMID 19757966. .
[30] "Vision in Animals: What Do Dogs and Cats See?" (http:/ / www. malamutehealth. org/ articles/ eye_vision. htm#color).
malamutehealth.org. .
[31] Genetics, Vol. 153, 919-932, October 1999, Copyright © 1999. The Molecular Genetics of Red and Green Color Vision in Mammals Shozo
Yokoyamaa and F. Bernhard Radlwimmera.
[32] Jacobs G. H., Deegan J. F. (2001). "Photopigments and color vision in New World monkeys from the family Atelidae". Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London, Series B 268 (1468): 695–702. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1421.
[33] Jacobs G. H., Deegan J. F., Neitz , Neitz J., Crognale M. A. (1993). "Photopigments and color vision in the nocturnal monkey, Aotus".
Vision Research 33 (13): 1773–1783. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(93)90168-V. PMID 8266633.
[34] Mollon J. D., Bowmaker J. K., Jacobs G. H. (1984). "Variations of color vision in a New World primate can be explained by polymorphism
of retinal photopigments". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 222 (1228): 373–399. doi:10.1098/rspb.1984.0071.
[35] Sternberg, Robert J. (2006): Cognitive Psychology. 4th Ed. Thomson Wadsworth.
[36] Arrese CA, Beazley LD, Neumeyer C (March 2006). "Behavioural evidence for marsupial trichromacy". Curr. Biol. 16 (6): R193–4.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.036. PMID 16546067.
[37] Pinker, Steven (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: Norton. p. 191. ISBN 0-393-04535-8.
[38] Koyanagi, M.; Nagata, T.; Katoh, K.; Yamashita, S.; Tokunaga, F. (2008). "Molecular Evolution of Arthropod Color Vision Deduced from
Multiple Opsin Genes of Jumping Spiders". Journal of Molecular Evolution 66 (2): 130–137. doi:10.1007/s00239-008-9065-9.
PMID 18217181.
[39] David Hambling (May 30, 2002). "Let the light shine in: You don't have to come from another planet to see ultraviolet light" (http:/ / www.
guardian. co. uk/ science/ 2002/ may/ 30/ medicalscience. research). EducationGuardian.co.uk. .
[40] Jacobs GH, Neitz J, Deegan JF (1991). "Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light" (http:/ / www. nature. com/
nature/ journal/ v353/ n6345/ abs/ 353655a0. html). Nature 353 (6345): 655–6. doi:10.1038/353655a0. PMID 1922382. .
[41] FJ Varela, AG Palacios, and TM Goldsmith (1993). Bischof, Hans-Joachim; Zeigler, H. Philip. ed. Vision, brain, and behavior in birds.
Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. pp. 77–94. ISBN 0-262-24036-X.
[42] IC Cuthill, JC Partridge, ATD Bennett, SC Church, NS Hart, and S Hunt (2000). "Ultraviolet Vision in Birds". Advances in the Study of
Behavior. 29. pp. 159–214.
[43] Süsstrunk, Sabine. Chromatic Adaptation (http:/ / ivrgwww. epfl. ch/ research/ past_topics/ chromatic_adaptation. html)
[44] Lindbloom, Bruce. Chromatic Adaptation (http:/ / www. brucelindbloom. com/ Eqn_ChromAdapt. html)
[45] Fairchild, Mark D. (2005). "8. Chromatic Adaptation" (http:/ / books. google. com/ ?id=8_TxzK2B-5MC& pg=PA146& dq="chromatic+
adaptation"). Color Appearance Models. Wiley. p. 146. ISBN 0-470-01216-1. .
Color vision 64
External links
• Peter Gouras, "Color Vision" (http://webvision.med.utah.edu/book/part-vii-color-vision/color-vision/),
Webvision, University of Utah School of Medicine, May 2009.
• Kenneth R. Koehler, "Spectral Sensitivity of the Eye" (http://www.rwc.uc.edu/koehler/biophys/6d.html),
College Physics for Students of Biology and Chemistry, University of Cincinnati Raymond Walters College,
1996.
• James T. Fulton, "The Human is a Blocked Tetrachromat" (http://www.neuronresearch.net/vision/files/
tetrachromat.htm), Neural Concepts, July 2009.
• Vurdlak, "Mega Color Blindness Test" (http://www.moillusions.com/2009/03/mega-color-blindness-test.
html), Mighty Optical Illusions, March 2009.
• Clive Maxfield and Alvin Brown, "Color Vision: One of Nature's Wonders" (http://www.diycalculator.com/
sp-cvision.shtml), DIYCalculator.com, 2006.
• Egopont, "Color Vision Test" (http://www.egopont.com/colorvision.php).
• Bruce McEvoy (2008). "Color vision" (http://www.handprint.com/LS/CVS/color.html). Retrieved
2012-03-30.
Visual perception
Visual perception is the ability to interpret the surrounding environment by processing information that is contained
in visible light. The resulting perception is also known as eyesight, sight, or vision (adjectival form: visual, optical,
or ocular). The various physiological components involved in vision are referred to collectively as the visual system,
and are the focus of much research in psychology, cognitive science, neuroscience, and molecular biology.
Visual system
The visual system in humans and animals allows individuals to assimilate information from the environment. The act
of seeing starts when the lens of the eye focuses an image of its surroundings onto a light-sensitive membrane in the
back of the eye, called the retina. The retina is actually part of the brain that is isolated to serve as a transducer for
the conversion of patterns of light into neuronal signals. The lens of the eye focuses light on the photoreceptive cells
of the retina, which detect the photons of light and respond by producing neural impulses. These signals are
processed in a hierarchical fashion by different parts of the brain, from the retina upstream to central ganglia in the
brain.
Note that up until now much of the above paragraph could apply to octopi, molluscs, worms, insects and things more
primitive; anything with a more concentrated nervous system and better eyes than say a jellyfish. However, the
following applies to mammals generally and birds (in modified form): The retina in these more complex animals
sends fibers (the optic nerve) to the lateral geniculate nucleus, to the primary and secondary visual cortex of the
brain. Signals from the retina can also travel directly from the retina to the superior colliculus.
Visual perception 65
Early studies
There were two major ancient Greek
schools, providing a primitive explanation
of how vision is carried out in the body.
The first was the "emission theory" which
maintained that vision occurs when rays
emanate from the eyes and are intercepted
by visual objects. If an object was seen
directly it was by 'means of rays' coming out
of the eyes and again falling on the object. A
refracted image was, however, seen by
'means of rays' as well, which came out of
the eyes, traversed through the air, and after
refraction, fell on the visible object which
was sighted as the result of the movement of The visual dorsal stream (green) and ventral stream (purple) are shown. Much of
the rays from the eye. This theory was the human cerebral cortex is involved in vision.
The second school advocated the so-called 'intro-mission' approach which sees vision as coming from something
entering the eyes representative of the object. With its main propagators Aristotle, Galen and their followers, this
theory seems to have some contact with modern theories of what vision really is, but it remained only a speculation
lacking any experimental foundation.
Both schools of thought relied upon the principle that "like is only known by like", and thus upon the notion that the
eye was composed of some "internal fire" which interacted with the "external fire" of visible light and made vision
possible. Plato makes this assertion in his dialogue Timaeus, as does Aristotle, in his De Sensu.[1]
Alhazen (965 – c. 1040) carried out many investigations and
experiments on visual perception, extended the work of Ptolemy on
binocular vision, and commented on the anatomical works of
Galen.[2][3]
Leonardo DaVinci (1452–1519) was the first to recognize the special
optical qualities of the eye. He wrote "The function of the human eye
... was described by a large number of authors in a certain way. But I
found it to be completely different." His main experimental finding
Leonardo DaVinci: The eye has a central line and was that there is only a distinct and clear vision at the line of sight, the
everything that reaches the eye through this optical line that ends at the fovea. Although he did not use these words
central line can be seen distinctly.
literally he actually is the father of the modern distinction between
foveal and peripheral vision.
Visual perception 66
Unconscious inference
Hermann von Helmholtz is often credited with the first study of visual perception in modern times. Helmholtz
examined the human eye and concluded that it was, optically, rather poor. The poor-quality information gathered via
the eye seemed to him to make vision impossible. He therefore concluded that vision could only be the result of
some form of unconscious inferences: a matter of making assumptions and conclusions from incomplete data, based
on previous experiences.
Inference requires prior experience of the world.
Examples of well-known assumptions, based on visual experience, are:
• light comes from above
• objects are normally not viewed from below
• faces are seen (and recognized) upright.[4]
• closer objects can block the view of more distant objects, but not vice versa
The study of visual illusions (cases when the inference process goes wrong) has yielded much insight into what sort
of assumptions the visual system makes.
Another type of the unconscious inference hypothesis (based on probabilities) has recently been revived in so-called
Bayesian studies of visual perception.[5] Proponents of this approach consider that the visual system performs some
form of Bayesian inference to derive a perception from sensory data. Models based on this idea have been used to
describe various visual subsystems, such as the perception of motion or the perception of depth.[6][7] The "wholly
empirical theory of perception" is a related and newer approach that rationalizes visual perception without explicitly
invoking Bayesian formalisms.[8]
Gestalt theory
Gestalt psychologists working primarily in the 1930s and 1940s raised many of the research questions that are
studied by vision scientists today. The Gestalt Laws of Organization have guided the study of how people perceive
visual components as organized patterns or wholes, instead of many different parts. Gestalt is a German word that
partially translates to "configuration or pattern" along with "whole or emergent structure." According to this theory,
there are six main factors that determine how the visual system automatically groups elements into patterns:
Proximity, Similarity, Closure, Symmetry, Common Fate (i.e. common motion), and Continuity.
the same area of both retinas. This results in a single focused image. Saccadic movements is the type of eye
movement that is used to rapidly scan a particular scene/image. Lastly, pursuit movement is used to follow objects
in motion.[13]
In the 1970s David Marr developed a multi-level theory of vision, which analysed the process of vision at different
levels of abstraction. In order to focus on the understanding of specific problems in vision, he identified three levels
of analysis: the computational, algorithmic and implementational levels. Many vision scientists, including Tomaso
Poggio, have embraced these levels of analysis and employed them to further characterize vision from a
computational perspective.
The computational level addresses, at a high level of abstraction, the problems that the visual system must overcome.
The algorithmic level attempts to identify the strategy that may be used to solve these problems. Finally, the
implementational level attempts to explain how solutions to these problems are realized in neural circuitry.
Marr suggested that it is possible to investigate vision at any of these levels independently. Marr described vision as
proceeding from a two-dimensional visual array (on the retina) to a three-dimensional description of the world as
output. His stages of vision include:
• a 2D or primal sketch of the scene, based on feature extraction of fundamental components of the scene, including
edges, regions, etc. Note the similarity in concept to a pencil sketch drawn quickly by an artist as an impression.
• a 2½ D sketch of the scene, where textures are acknowledged, etc. Note the similarity in concept to the stage in
drawing where an artist highlights or shades areas of a scene, to provide depth.
• a 3 D model, where the scene is visualized in a continuous, 3-dimensional map.[17]
Visual perception 68
Transduction
Transduction is the process through which energy from environmental stimuli is converted to neural activity for the
brain to understand and process. The back of the eye contains three different cell layers; Photoreceptor layer, Bipolar
cell layer and Ganglion cell layer. The photoreceptor layer is at the very back and contains rod photoreceptors and
cone photoreceptors. Cones are responsible for colour perception. There are three different cones: red, green and
blue. Photoreceptors contain within them photopigments, composed of two molecules. There are 3 specific
photopigments (each with their own colour) that respond to specific wavelengths of light. When the appropriate
wavelength of light hits the photoreceptor, its photopigment splits into two, which sends a message to the bipolar cell
layer, which in turn sends a message to the ganglion cells, which then send the information through the optic nerve
to the brain. If the appropriate photopigment is not in the proper photoreceptor (for example, a green photopigment
inside a red cone), a condition called colour blindness will occur.[18]
Opponent Process
Transduction involves chemical messages sent from the photoreceptors to the bipolar cells to the ganglion cells.
Several photoreceptors may send their information to one ganglion cell. There are two types of ganglion cells: red /
green and yellow/blue. These neuron cells consistently fire – even when not stimulated. The brain interprets different
colours (and with a lot of information, an image) when the rate of firing of these neurons alters. Red light stimulates
the red cone, which in turn stimulates the red/green ganglion cell. Likewise, green light stimulates the green cone,
which stimulates the red/green ganglion cell and blue light stimulates the blue cone which stimulates the yellow/blue
ganglion cell. The rate of firing of the ganglion cells is increased when it is signalled by one cone and decreased
(inhibited) when it is signalled by the other cone. The first colour in the name if the ganglion cell is the colour that
excites it and the second is the colour that inhibits it. I.e.: A red cone would excite the red/green ganglion cell and the
green cone would inhibit the red/green ganglion cell. This is an opponent process. If the rate of firing of a red/green
ganglion cell is increased, the brain would know that the light was red, if the rate was decreased, the brain would
know that the colour of the light was green.[18]
References
[1] Finger, Stanley (1994). Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press.
ISBN 0-19-506503-4. OCLC 27151391.
[2] Howard, I (1996). "Alhazen's neglected discoveries of visual phenomena". Perception 25 (10): 1203–1217. doi:10.1068/p251203.
PMID 9027923.
[3] Omar Khaleefa (1999). "Who Is the Founder of Psychophysics and Experimental Psychology?" (http:/ / i-epistemology. net/ attachments/
637_V16N2 Summer 99 - Khaleefa - Who is the Founder of Psychophysics and Experimental Psychology. pdf). American Journal of Islamic
Social Sciences 16 (2). .
[4] Hans-Werner Hunziker, (2006) Im Auge des Lesers: foveale und periphere Wahrnehmung - vom Buchstabieren zur Lesefreude [In the eye of
the reader: foveal and peripheral perception - from letter recognition to the joy of reading] Transmedia Stäubli Verlag Zürich 2006 ISBN
978-3-7266-0068-6
[5] Stone JV, "Footprints Sticking Out of the Sand (Part II): Children’s Bayesian Priors For Lighting Direction and Convexity", Perception,
40(2), pp175-190, 2011.
[6] Mamassian, Landy & Maloney (2002)
[7] A Primer on Probabilistic Approaches to Visual Perception (http:/ / www. purveslab. net/ research/ primer. html)
[8] The Wholly Empirical Theory of Perception (http:/ / www. purveslab. net/ research. html)
Visual perception 69
[9] Taylor, Stanford E. (1965). "Eye Movements in Reading: Facts and Fallacies". American Educational Research Journal 2 (4): 187.
doi:10.2307/1161646. ISSN 00028312.
[10] Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye movements and vision, Plenum Press, New York
[11] Hunziker, H. W. (1970). Visuelle Informationsaufnahme und Intelligenz: Eine Untersuchung über die Augenfixationen beim Problemlösen.
Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie und ihre Anwendungen, 1970, 29, Nr 1/2
[12] Cohen, A. S. (1983). Informationsaufnahme beim Befahren von Kurven, Psychologie für die Praxis 2/83, Bulletin der Schweizerischen
Stiftung für Angewandte Psychologie
[13] Carlson, Neil R. (2010). Psychology the Science of Behaviour. Toronto Ontario: Pearson Canada Inc.. pp. 140–141.
[14] Alex Huk. (1999) "Object and Face Recognition: Lecture Notes." pp. 5 (http:/ / www. saylor. org/ site/ wp-content/ uploads/ 2011/ 08/
Psych306-Reading-4. 6. pdf)
[15] A.K.Beeharee - http:/ / www. cs. ucl. ac. uk/ staff/ A. Beeharee/ research. htm
[16] Bruce, V., Green, P. & Georgeson, M. (1996). Visual perception: Physiology, psychology and ecology (3rd ed.). LEA. pp. 110.
[17] Marr, D (1982). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. MIT Press.
[18] Carlson, Neil R.; Heth, C. Donald (2010). "5". Psychology the science of behaviour (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey, USA:
Pearson Education Inc.. pp. 138–145. ISBN 978-0-205-64524-4.
External links
• Visual Perception 3 - Cultural and Environmental Factors (http://www.aber.ac.uk/media/Modules/MC10220/
visper03.html)
• Gestalt Laws (http://www.sapdesignguild.org/resources/optical_illusions/gestalt_laws.html)
• Summary of Kosslyn et al.'s theory of high-level vision (http://develintel.blogspot.com/2006/01/
kosslyns-cognitive-architecture.html)
• The Organization of the Retina and Visual System (http://webvision.med.utah.edu/)
• Reference info on aritificial visual perception (http://www.diaplous.com/id4.html)
• Dr Trippy's Sensorium (http://dr.trippy.googlepages.com) A website dedicated to the study of the human
sensorium and organisational behaviour
• Effect of Detail on Visual Perception (http://demonstrations.wolfram.com/EffectOfDetailOnVisualPerception/
) by Jon McLoone, the Wolfram Demonstrations Project.
• The Joy of Visual Perception (http://www.yorku.ca/eye/toc.htm) An excellent resource on the eye's
perception abilities.
• VisionScience. An Internet Resource for Research in Human and Animal Vision (http://www.visionscience.
com) A most comprehensive collection of resources in vision science and perception.
• Vision and Psychophysics. (http://www.cis.rit.edu/people/faculty/montag/vandplite/course.html) A quality
account of many aspects of vision. However, some parts are missing.
• Visibility in Social Theory and Social Research. (http://www.palgrave.com/products/title.aspx?pid=385931)
An inquiry into the cognitive and social meanings of visibility.
70
Visual Color
List of colors
The following is a list of colors. A number of the color swatches
below are taken from domain-specific naming schemes such as X11 or
HTML4. RGB values are given for each swatch because such
standards are defined in terms of the sRGB color space. It is not
possible to accurately convert many of these swatches to CMYK
values because of the differing gamuts of the two spaces, but the color
management systems built into operating systems and image editing
software attempt such conversions as accurately as possible.
Color is an important part of the visual arts,
The HSV (hue, saturation, value) color space values, also known as fashion, interior design and many other fields and
HSB (hue, saturation, brightness), and the hex triplets (for HTML web disciplines.
colors) are also given in the following table. Colors that appear on the
web-safe color palette—which includes the sixteen named colors—are noted.[1] (Those four named colors
corresponding to the neutral grays have no hue value, which is effectively ignored—i.e., left blank.)
• A
• B
• C
• D
• E
• F
• G
• H
• I
• J
• K
• L
• M
• N
• O
• P
• Q
• R
• S
• T
• U
• V
• W
• X
• Y
• Z
• White
• Pink
List of colors 71
• Red
• Orange
• Brown
• Yellow
• Gray
• Green
• Cyan
• Blue
• Violet
• Web colors
• See also
• References
Color names
Color Name Hex Red Green Blue Hue Satur Light Satur Value W3C
(RGB) (RGB) (RGB) (RGB) (HSL/HSV) (HSL) (HSL) (HSV) (HSV) name
Aero #7CB9E8 49% 73% 91% 206° 70% 70% 47% 91%
Aero blue #C9FFE5 79% 100% 90% 151° 100% 89% 21% 100%
African violet #B284BE 70% 52% 75% 288° 31% 63% 31% 75%
Air Force blue (RAF) #5D8AA8 36% 54% 66% 204° 30% 51% 45% 66%
Air Force blue (USAF) #00308F 0% 19% 56% 220° 100% 28% 100% 56%
Air superiority blue #72A0C1 45% 63% 76% 205° 39% 60% 41% 76%
Alabama Crimson #A32638 64% 15% 22% 350° 62% 39% 80% 60%
Alice blue #F0F8FF 94% 97% 100% 208° 100% 97% 6% 100%
Alizarin crimson #E32636 89% 15% 21% 355° 77% 52% 83% 89%
Alloy orange #C46210 77% 38% 6% 27° 85% 42% 92% 77%
Almond #EFDECD 94% 87% 80% 30° 52% 87% 14% 94%
Amaranth #E52B50 90% 17% 31% 348° 78% 53% 81% 90%
Amazon #3B7A57 23% 48% 34% 147° 35% 36% 52% 48%
SAE/ECE Amber (color) #FF7E00 100% 49% 0% 30° 100% 50% 100% 100%
American rose #FF033E 100% 1% 24% 345° 100% 51% 99% 87%
Amethyst #9966CC 60% 40% 80% 270° 50% 60% 50% 80%
Android green #A4C639 64% 78% 22% 74° 55% 50% 71% 78%
Antique brass #CD9575 80% 58% 46% 22° 47% 63% 43% 80%
Antique bronze #665D1E 40% 36% 12% 53° 55% 26% 71% 40%
Antique fuchsia #915C83 57% 36% 51% 316° 22% 47% 37% 57%
Antique ruby #841B2D 52% 11% 18% 350° 66% 31% 80% 52%
Antique white #FAEBD7 98% 92% 84% 34° 78% 91% 14% 98%
List of colors 72
Apple green #8DB600 55% 71% 0% 74° 100% 36% 100% 71%
Apricot #FBCEB1 98% 81% 69% 24° 90% 84% 29% 98%
Aquamarine #7FFFD4 50% 100% 83% 160° 100% 75% 50% 100%
Army green #4B5320 29% 33% 13% 69° 44% 23% 61% 33%
Arsenic #3B444B 23% 27% 29% 206° 12% 26% 21% 29%
Arylide yellow #E9D66B 91% 84% 42% 51° 74% 67% 54% 91%
Asparagus #87A96B 53% 66% 42% 93° 27% 54% 37% 66%
Atomic tangerine #FF9966 100% 60% 40% 20° 100% 70% 60% 100%
Azure mist/web #F0FFFF 94% 100% 100% 180° 100% 97% 6% 100%
Baby blue #89CFF0 54% 81% 94% 199° 77% 74% 43% 94%
Baby blue eyes #A1CAF1 63% 79% 95% 209° 74% 79% 33% 95%
Baby pink #F4C2C2 96% 76% 76% 30° 69% 86% 21% 96%
Baby powder #FEFEFA 100% 100% 98% 60° 67% 99% 2% 100%
Baker-Miller pink #FF91AF 100% 57% 69% 344° 100% 78% 43% 100%
Ball blue #21ABCD 13% 67% 80% 192° 72% 47% 84% 80%
Banana Mania #FAE7B5 98% 91% 71% 43° 87% 85% 28% 98%
Banana yellow #FFE135 100% 88% 21% 51° 100% 60% 79% 100%
Barbie pink #E0218A 88% 13% 54% 327° 76% 50% 85% 88%
Bazaar #98777B 60% 47% 48% 353° 14% 53% 22% 60%
Beau blue #BCD4E6 74% 83% 90% 206° 46% 82% 18% 90%
Beaver #9F8170 62% 51% 44% 22° 20% 53% 35% 63%
Beige #F5F5DC 96% 96% 86% 60° 56% 91% 10% 96%
B'dazzled Blue #2E5894 18% 35% 58% 215° 53% 38% 69% 58%
Big dip o’ruby #9C2542 61% 15% 26% 345° 62% 38% 76% 61%
Bisque #FFE4C4 100% 89% 77% 33° 100% 88% 23% 100%
Bistre #3D2B1F 24% 17% 12% 24° 33% 18% 49% 24%
Bistre brown #967117 59% 44% 9% 43° 73% 34% 85% 59%
Bitter lemon #CAE00D 79% 88% 5% 66° 89% 47% 94% 88%
Bitter lime #BFFF00 39% 55% 7% 79° 78% 31% 78% 84%
List of colors 73
Bittersweet shimmer #BF4F51 75% 31% 32% 359° 47% 53% 59% 75%
Black leather jacket #253529 15% 21% 16% 135° 18% 18% 6% 18%
Black olive #3B3C36 23% 24% 21% 70° 5% 22% 10% 24%
Blanched almond #FFEBCD 100% 92% 80% 36° 100% 90% 20% 100%
Blast-off bronze #A57164 65% 44% 39% 12° 27% 52% 39% 65%
Bleu de France #318CE7 19% 55% 91% 210° 79% 55% 79% 91%
Blizzard Blue #ACE5EE 67% 90% 93% 188° 66% 80% 28% 93%
Blond #FAF0BE 98% 94% 75% 50° 86% 86% 24% 98%
Blue (Crayola) #1F75FE 12% 46% 100% 213° 99% 56% 99% 100%
Blue (Munsell) #0093AF 0% 58% 69% 190° 100% 34% 100% 68%
Blue (NCS) #0087BD 0% 53% 74% 197° 100% 37% 100% 74%
Blue (pigment) #333399 20% 20% 60% 240° 50% 40% 67% 60%
Blue (RYB) #0247FE 1% 28% 100% 224° 99% 50% 99% 99%
Blue Bell #A2A2D0 64% 64% 82% 240° 33% 73% 22% 81%
Blue-gray #6699CC 40% 60% 80% 210° 50% 60% 50% 80%
Blue sapphire #126180 7% 38% 50% 197° 75% 29% 86% 50%
Blue-violet #8A2BE2 54% 17% 89% 266° 76% 53% 81% 89%
Blueberry #4F86F7 31% 53% 97% 220° 91% 64% 68% 97%
Bluebonnet #1C1CF0 11% 11% 94% 240° 88% 53% 83% 94%
Blush #DE5D83 87% 36% 51% 342° 66% 62% 58% 87%
Bole #79443B 47% 27% 23% 30° 34% 35% 24% 34%
Bondi blue #0095B6 0% 58% 71% 191° 100% 36% 100% 71%
Bone #E3DAC9 89% 85% 79% 48° 32% 84% 30% 95%
Bottle green #006A4E 0% 42% 31% 164° 100% 21% 100% 41%
Boysenberry #873260 53% 20% 38% 328° 46% 36% 63% 53%
Brandeis blue #0070FF 0% 44% 100% 214° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Brass #B5A642 71% 65% 26% 52° 47% 48% 64% 71%
Brick red #CB4154 80% 25% 33% 352° 57% 53% 68% 80%
Bright cerulean #1DACD6 11% 67% 84% 194° 76% 48% 86% 84%
Bright green #66FF00 40% 100% 0% 96° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Bright lavender #BF94E4 75% 58% 89% 272° 60% 74% 35% 89%
Bright maroon #C32148 76% 13% 28% 345° 71% 45% 75% 38%
Bright pink #FF007F 100% 0% 50% 330° 100% 50% 100% 100%
List of colors 74
Bright turquoise #08E8DE 3% 91% 87% 177° 93% 47% 97% 91%
Bright ube #D19FE8 82% 62% 91% 281° 61% 77% 31% 91%
Brilliant lavender #F4BBFF 96% 73% 100% 290° 100% 87% 27% 100%
Brilliant rose #FF55A3 100% 33% 64% 332° 100% 67% 67% 100%
Brink pink #FB607F 98% 38% 50% 348° 95% 68% 62% 98%
British racing green #004225 0% 26% 15% 154° 100% 13% 100% 26%
Bronze #CD7F32 80% 50% 20% 30° 61% 50% 76% 80%
Bronze Yellow #737000 45% 44% 0% 58° 100% 23% 100% 45%
Brown (traditional) #964B00 59% 29% 0% 30° 100% 29% 100% 59%
Brown (web) #A52A2A 65% 16% 16% 0° 59% 41% 75% 65%
Brown-nose #6B4423 40% 27% 14% 28° 49% 27% 67% 42%
Brunswick green #1B4D3E 11% 30% 24% 162° 48% 20% 65% 30%
Bubble gum #FFC1CC 100% 76% 80% 349° 100% 88% 23% 99%
Buff #F0DC82 94% 86% 51% 49° 79% 73% 46% 94%
Burlywood #DEB887 87% 72% 53% 34° 57% 70% 39% 87%
Burnt orange #CC5500 80% 33% 0% 25° 100% 40% 100% 80%
Burnt sienna #E97451 91% 45% 32% 14° 78% 62% 65% 91%
Burnt umber #8A3324 54% 20% 14% 9° 59% 34% 74% 54%
Byzantine #BD33A4 74% 20% 64% 311° 57% 47% 73% 74%
Byzantium #702963 44% 16% 39% 311° 46% 30% 63% 44%
Cadet #536872 33% 41% 45% 206° 16% 39% 31% 47%
Cadet blue #5F9EA0 37% 62% 63% 182° 26% 50% 41% 63%
Cadet grey #91A3B0 57% 64% 69% 205° 16% 63% 18% 69%
Cadmium green #006B3C 0% 42% 24% 154° 100% 21% 100% 42%
Cadmium orange #ED872D 93% 53% 18% 28° 84% 55% 81% 93%
Cadmium red #E30022 89% 0% 13% 351° 100% 45% 100% 89%
Cadmium yellow #FFF600 100% 96% 0% 34° 100% 50% 93% 100%
Café au lait #A67B5B 65% 48% 36% 26° 30% 50% 45% 65%
Café noir #4B3621 29% 21% 13% 30° 39% 21% 56% 29%
Cal Poly green #1E4D2B 12% 30% 17% 137° 44% 21% 61% 30%
Cambridge Blue #A3C1AD 64% 76% 68% 140° 20% 70% 16% 76%
Camel #C19A6B 76% 60% 42% 33° 41% 59% 45% 76%
Cameo pink #EFBBCC 94% 73% 80% 340° 62% 84% 22% 94%
Camouflage green #78866B 47% 53% 42% 91° 11% 47% 20% 53%
Canary yellow #FFEF00 100% 94% 0% 56° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Candy pink #E4717A 89% 44% 48% 355° 68% 67% 50% 89%
Caput mortuum #592720 35% 15% 13% 7° 47% 24% 64% 35%
Cardinal #C41E3A 77% 12% 23% 350° 74% 44% 85% 77%
Caribbean green #00CC99 0% 80% 60% 150° 100% 40% 100% 44%
Carmine (M&P) #D70040 84% 0% 25% 342° 100% 42% 100% 84%
Carmine pink #EB4C42 92% 30% 26% 4° 81% 59% 72% 92%
Carmine red #FF0038 100% 0% 22% 347° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Carnation pink #FFA6C9 100% 65% 79% 336° 100% 83% 35% 100%
Carolina blue #99BADD 60% 73% 87% 211° 50% 73% 31% 87%
Carrot orange #ED9121 93% 57% 13% 33° 85% 53% 86% 93%
Castleton green #00563F 0% 34% 25% 164° 100% 17% 100% 34%
Catalina blue #062A78 2% 16% 47% 221° 91% 25% 95% 47%
Catawba #703642 44% 21% 26% 348° 35% 33% 52% 44%
Cedar Chest #C95A49 79% 35% 29% 8° 54% 54% 64% 79%
Ceil #92A1CF 57% 63% 81% 225° 39% 69% 30% 81%
Celadon #ACE1AF 67% 88% 69% 174° 47% 78% 47% 73%
Celadon blue #007BA7 0% 48% 65% 196° 100% 33% 100% 65%
Celadon green #2F847C 18% 52% 49% 123° 48% 35% 24% 88%
Celeste (colour) #B2FFFF 70% 100% 100% 180° 100% 85% 30% 100%
Celestial blue #4997D0 29% 59% 82% 205° 59% 55% 65% 81%
Cerise #DE3163 87% 19% 39% 343° 72% 53% 78% 87%
Cerise pink #EC3B83 93% 23% 51% 336° 82% 58% 75% 93%
Cerulean blue #2A52BE 16% 32% 75% 224° 64% 46% 78% 75%
Cerulean frost #6D9BC3 43% 61% 76% 208° 42% 60% 44% 77%
Chamoisee #A0785A 63% 47% 35% 26° 28% 49% 44% 63%
Champagne #F7E7CE 97% 91% 81% 37° 72% 89% 17% 97%
Charcoal #36454F 21% 27% 31% 204° 19% 26% 31% 31%
Charleston green #232B2B 14% 17% 17% 180° 10% 15% 19% 17%
Charm pink #E68FAC 90% 56% 67% 333° 64% 73% 50% 87%
Chartreuse (traditional) #DFFF00 87% 100% 0% 68° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Chartreuse (web) #7FFF00 50% 100% 0% 90° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Cherry #DE3163 87% 19% 39% 343° 72% 53% 78% 87%
Cherry blossom pink #FFB7C5 100% 72% 77% 348° 100% 86% 28% 100%
List of colors 76
Chestnut #954535 56% 27% 21% 10° 46% 39% 54% 68%
China pink #DE6FA1 87% 44% 63% 333° 63% 65% 50% 87%
China rose #A8516E 66% 32% 43% 340° 35% 49% 52% 66%
Chinese red #AA381E 67% 22% 12% 11° 70% 39% 82% 67%
Chinese violet #856088 52% 38% 53% 296° 17% 46% 29% 53%
Chocolate (traditional) #7B3F00 48% 25% 0% 31° 100% 24% 100% 48%
Chocolate (web) #D2691E 82% 41% 12% 25° 75% 47% 86% 82%
Chrome yellow #FFA700 100% 65% 0% 40° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Cinereous #98817B 60% 51% 48% 12° 12% 54% 19% 60%
Cinnamon #D2691E 82% 41% 12% 25° 75% 47% 86% 82%
Citron #9FA91F 62% 66% 12% 64° 69% 39% 82% 66%
Classic rose #FBCCE7 98% 80% 91% 333° 86% 89% 100% 20%
Cocoa brown #D2691E 82% 41% 12% 25° 75% 47% 86% 82%
Coconut #965A3E 59% 35% 24% 19° 42% 42% 59% 59%
Coffee #6F4E37 44% 31% 22% 25° 34% 33% 51% 44%
Columbia blue #9BDDFF 61% 87% 100% 200° 100% 80% 39% 100%
Congo pink #F88379 97% 51% 47% 5° 90% 72% 51% 97%
Cool black #002E63 0% 18% 39% 212° 100% 19% 100% 39%
Cool grey #8C92AC 55% 57% 67% 229° 16% 61% 19% 68%
Copper #B87333 72% 45% 20% 29° 57% 46% 72% 72%
Copper (Crayola) #DA8A67 85% 54% 40% 18° 61% 63% 53% 85%
Copper penny #AD6F69 68% 44% 41% 5° 29% 55% 39% 68%
Copper red #CB6D51 80% 43% 32% 14° 54% 56% 60% 80%
Copper rose #996666 60% 40% 40% 0° 20% 50% 33% 60%
Coral #FF7F50 100% 50% 31% 16° 100% 66% 69% 100%
Coral pink #F88379 97% 51% 47% 5° 90% 72% 51% 97%
Coral red #FF4040 100% 25% 25% 0° 100% 63% 75% 100%
Cordovan #893F45 54% 25% 27% 337° 37% 39% 89% 94%
Corn #FBEC5D 98% 93% 36% 54° 95% 68% 63% 98%
Cornell Red #B31B1B 70% 11% 11% 0° 74% 40% 85% 70%
Cornflower blue #6495ED 39% 58% 93% 219° 79% 66% 58% 93%
Cornsilk #FFF8DC 100% 97% 86% 48° 100% 93% 14% 100%
Cosmic latte #FFF8E7 100% 97% 91% 42° 100% 95% 9% 100%
Cotton candy #FFBCD9 100% 74% 85% 334° 100% 87% 26% 100%
List of colors 77
Cream #FFFDD0 100% 99% 82% 57° 100% 91% 18% 100%
Crimson glory #BE0032 75% 0% 20% 356° 100% 37% 100% 75%
Cyan #00FFFF 0% 100% 100% 180° 100% 50% 100% 100% Cyan
Cyan (process) #00B7EB 0% 72% 92% 193° 100% 46% 100% 92%
Cyber grape #58427C 35% 26% 49% 263° 31% 37% 47% 49%
Daffodil #FFFF31 100% 100% 19% 60° 100% 60% 81% 100%
Dandelion #F0E130 94% 88% 19% 55° 87% 56% 80% 94%
Dark blue-gray #666699 40% 40% 60% 240° 20% 50% 33% 60%
Dark brown #654321 40% 26% 13% 30° 51% 26% 67% 40%
Dark byzantium #5D3954 36% 22% 33% 315° 24% 29% 39% 37%
Dark candy apple red #A40000 64% 0% 0% 0° 100% 32% 100% 64%
Dark cerulean #08457E 3% 27% 49% 209° 88% 26% 94% 49%
Dark chestnut #986960 60% 41% 38% 10° 23% 49% 37% 60%
Dark coral #CD5B45 80% 36% 27% 10° 58% 54% 66% 80%
Dark cyan #008B8B 0% 55% 55% 180° 100% 27% 24% 100%
Dark electric blue #536878 33% 41% 47% 180° 18% 40% 20% 25%
Dark goldenrod #B8860B 72% 53% 4% 43° 89% 38% 94% 72%
Dark green #013220 0% 20% 13% 158° 96% 10% 98% 20%
Dark imperial blue #00416A 0% 25% 42% 203° 100% 21% 100% 42%
Dark jungle green #1A2421 10% 14% 13% 120° 16% 12% 10% 10%
Dark khaki #BDB76B 74% 72% 42% 56° 38% 58% 43% 74%
Dark lava #483C32 28% 24% 20% 27° 18% 24% 31% 28%
Dark lavender #734F96 45% 31% 59% 270° 31% 45% 47% 59%
Dark magenta #8B008B 55% 0% 55% 300° 100% 27% 100% 55%
Dark midnight blue #003366 0% 20% 40% 210° 100% 20% 100% 40%
Dark moss green #4A5D23 29% 36% 14% 80° 45% 25% 62% 37%
Dark olive green #556B2F 33% 42% 18% 82° 39% 30% 56% 42%
Dark orange #FF8C00 100% 55% 0% 34° 100% 50% 100% 94%
Dark orchid #9932CC 60% 20% 80% 280° 61% 50% 75% 80%
Dark pastel blue #779ECB 47% 62% 80% 212° 45% 63% 41% 80%
Dark pastel green #03C03C 1% 75% 24% 138° 97% 38% 98% 75%
Dark pastel purple #966FD6 59% 44% 84% 263° 56% 64% 48% 84%
Dark pastel red #C23B22 76% 23% 13% 9° 70% 45% 82% 76%
Dark pink #E75480 91% 33% 50% 342° 75% 62% 64% 91%
Dark powder blue #003399 0% 20% 60% 220° 100% 30% 70% 60%
Dark raspberry #872657 53% 15% 34% 330° 56% 34% 72% 53%
List of colors 78
Dark salmon #E9967A 91% 59% 48% 15° 72% 70% 48% 91%
Dark scarlet #560319 34% 1% 10% 344° 93% 18% 97% 34%
Dark sea green #8FBC8F 56% 74% 56% 120° 25% 65% 24% 74%
Dark sky blue #8CBED6 55% 75% 84% 199° 47% 69% 35% 84%
Dark slate blue #483D8B 28% 24% 55% 248° 39% 39% 56% 55%
Dark slate gray #2F4F4F 18% 31% 31% 180° 25% 25% 41% 31%
Dark spring green #177245 9% 45% 27% 150° 66% 27% 80% 45%
Dark tan #918151 57% 51% 32% 45° 28% 44% 44% 57%
Dark tangerine #FFA812 100% 66% 7% 38° 100% 54% 93% 100%
Dark taupe #483C32 28% 24% 20% 30° 18% 24% 17% 34%
Dark terra cotta #CC4E5C 80% 31% 36% 354° 55% 55% 55% 55%
Dark turquoise #00CED1 0% 81% 82% 181° 100% 41% 100% 82%
Dark vanilla #D1BEA8 82% 75% 66% 32° 31% 74% 20% 82%
Dark violet #9400D3 58% 0% 83% 282° 100% 41% 100% 83%
Dark yellow #9B870C 61% 53% 5% 295° 86% 33% 92% 61%
Dartmouth green #00703C 0% 44% 24% 121° 100% 22% 90% 50%
Debian red #D70A53 84% 4% 33% 339° 91% 44% 95% 84%
Deep carmine #A9203E 66% 13% 24% 357° 68% 39% 100% 66%
Deep carmine pink #EF3038 94% 19% 22% 357° 86% 56% 80% 94%
Deep carrot orange #E9692C 91% 41% 17% 34° 81% 54% 76% 84%
Deep cerise #DA3287 85% 20% 53% 330° 69% 53% 77% 85%
Deep champagne #FAD6A5 98% 84% 65% 35° 90% 81% 34% 98%
Deep chestnut #B94E48 73% 31% 28% 3° 45% 50% 61% 73%
Deep coffee #704241 44% 26% 25% 1° 27% 35% 42% 44%
Deep fuchsia #C154C1 76% 33% 76% 300° 47% 54% 56% 76%
Deep jungle green #004B49 0% 29% 29% 120° 100% 15% 40% 40%
Deep lemon #F5C71A 96% 78% 30% 47° 89% 63% 89% 96%
Deep lilac #9955BB 60% 33% 73% 280° 43% 53% 55% 73%
Deep magenta #CC00CC 80% 0% 80% 300° 100% 40% 100% 80%
Deep mauve #D473D4 83% 45% 83% 300° 53% 64% 46% 83%
Deep moss green #355E3B 21% 37% 23% 129° 28% 29% 44% 37%
Deep peach #FFCBA4 100% 80% 64% 26° 100% 82% 36% 100%
Deep pink #FF1493 100% 8% 58% 328° 100% 54% 92% 100%
Deep ruby #843F5B 52% 25% 36% 336° 35% 38% 52% 52%
Deep saffron #FF9933 100% 60% 20% 30° 100% 60% 80% 100%
Deep sky blue #00BFFF 0% 75% 100% 195° 100% 50% 100% 100%
List of colors 79
Deep Space Sparkle #4A646C 29% 39% 42% 194° 19% 36% 32% 43%
Deep Taupe #7E5E60 49% 37% 38% 356° 14% 43% 25% 49%
Deep Tuscan red #66424D 40% 26% 30% 342° 21% 33% 35% 40%
Deer #BA8759 73% 53% 35% 28° 41% 54% 52% 73%
Desert #C19A6B 76% 60% 42% 33° 41% 59% 44% 76%
Desert sand #EDC9AF 93% 79% 69% 25° 63% 81% 26% 93%
Diamond #B9F2FF 73% 95% 100% 190° 100% 86% 100% 100%
Dirt #9B7653 61% 46% 33% 29° 30% 47% 47% 61%
Dodger blue #1E90FF 12% 56% 100% 210° 100% 56% 88% 100%
Dogwood rose #D71868 84% 9% 41% 330° 80% 47% 84% 82%
Dollar bill #85BB65 52% 73% 40% 98° 39% 56% 46% 73%
Dust storm #E5CCC9 90% 80% 79% 6° 35% 84% 12% 90%
Earth yellow #E1A95F 88% 66% 37% 34° 68% 63% 58% 88%
Ecru #C2B280 76% 70% 50% 45° 35% 63% 34% 76%
Eggplant #614051 38% 25% 32% 329° 21% 32% 34% 38%
Eggshell #F0EAD6 94% 92% 84% 46° 46% 89% 11% 94%
Egyptian blue #1034A6 6% 20% 65% 226° 82% 36% 90% 65%
Electric blue #7DF9FF 49% 98% 100% 183° 100% 75% 51% 100%
Electric crimson #FF003F 100% 0% 25% 345° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric cyan #00FFFF 0% 100% 100% 180° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric indigo #6F00FF 44% 0% 100% 266° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric lavender #F4BBFF 96% 73% 100% 290° 100% 87% 27% 100%
Electric lime #CCFF00 80% 100% 0% 72° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric purple #BF00FF 75% 0% 100% 285° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric ultramarine #3F00FF 25% 0% 100% 255° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric violet #8F00FF 56% 0% 100% 274° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Electric yellow #FFFF33 100% 100% 20% 60° 100% 60% 80% 100%
Emerald #50C878 31% 78% 47% 140° 52% 55% 60% 78%
English green #1B4D3E 11% 30% 24% 162° 48% 20% 65% 30%
English lavender #B48395 71% 51% 58% 338° 25% 61% 27% 71%
English red #AB4B52 67% 31% 32% 357° 37% 49% 54% 67%
English violet #563C5C 34% 24% 36% 289° 21% 30% 35% 36%
Eton blue #96C8A2 59% 78% 64% 134° 31% 69% 25% 78%
List of colors 80
Eucalyptus #44D7A8 27% 84% 66% 161° 65% 56% 68% 84%
Fallow #C19A6B 76% 60% 42% 45° 41% 59% 17% 23%
Fandango #B53389 71% 20% 54% 320° 56% 46% 72% 71%
Fandango pink #DE5285 87% 32% 52% 342° 68% 60% 63% 87%
Fashion fuchsia #F400A1 96% 0% 63% 320° 100% 48% 100% 96%
Fawn #E5AA70 90% 67% 44% 30° 69% 67% 51% 90%
Feldspar #FDD5B1 88% 71% 51% 33° 60% 69% 60% 56%
Fern green #4F7942 31% 47% 26% 106° 29% 37% 45% 47%
Field drab #6C541E 42% 33% 12% 42° 56% 27% 72% 42%
Fire engine red #CE2029 81% 13% 16% 0° 73% 47% 92% 80%
Flame #E25822 89% 35% 13% 17° 77% 51% 85% 89%
Flamingo pink #FC8EAC 99% 56% 67% 344° 95% 77% 44% 99%
Flattery #6B4423 40% 27% 14% 28° 49% 27% 67% 42%
Flavescent #F7E98E 97% 91% 56% 52° 87% 76% 41% 76%
Flax #EEDC82 93% 86% 51% 50° 76% 72% 45% 93%
Floral white #FFFAF0 100% 98% 94% 40° 100% 97% 6% 100%
Fluorescent orange #FFBF00 100% 75% 0% 45° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Fluorescent pink #FF1493 100% 8% 58% 328° 100% 54% 92% 100%
Fluorescent yellow #CCFF00 80% 100% 0% 72° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Forest green (traditional) #014421 0% 27% 13% 149° 97% 14% 99% 27%
Forest green (web) #228B22 13% 55% 13% 120° 61% 34% 76% 55%
French beige #A67B5B 65% 48% 36% 26° 30% 50% 45% 65%
French bistre #856D4D 52% 43% 30% 34° 27% 41% 42% 52%
French blue #0072BB 0% 45% 73% 203° 100% 37% 100% 73%
French lilac #86608E 53% 38% 56% 290° 19% 47% 32% 56%
French lime #9EFD38 62% 99% 22% 89° 98% 61% 78% 99%
French mauve #D473D4 83% 45% 83% 300° 53% 64% 46% 83%
French raspberry #C72C48 78% 17% 28% 351° 64% 48% 78% 78%
French rose #F64A8A 96% 29% 54% 338° 91% 63% 70% 96%
French sky blue #77B5FE 47% 71% 100% 213° 99% 73% 53% 100%
French wine #AC1E44 67% 12% 27% 344° 70% 40% 83% 68%
Fresh Air #A6E7FF 65% 91% 100% 196° 100% 83% 35% 100%
Fuchsia #FF00FF 100% 0% 100% 321° 100% 50% 100% 100% Fuchsia
Fuchsia (Crayola) #C154C1 76% 33% 76% 300° 47% 54% 56% 76%
List of colors 81
Fuchsia pink #FF77FF 100% 47% 100% 313° 100% 73% 53% 100%
Fuchsia rose #C74375 78% 26% 46% 337° 54% 52% 66% 78%
Fuzzy Wuzzy #CC6666 80% 40% 40% 0° 50% 60% 50% 80%
Ghost white #F8F8FF 97% 97% 100% 24° 100% 99% 3% 100%
Giants orange #FE5A1D 100% 35% 11% 16° 99% 56% 89% 100%
Glaucous #6082B6 38% 51% 71% 216° 37% 55% 47% 71%
Gold (metallic) #D4AF37 83% 69% 22% 46° 65% 52% 74% 83%
Gold (web) (Golden) #FFD700 100% 84% 0% 51° 100% 50% 100% 100% Gold
Gold Fusion #85754E 52% 46% 31% 43° 26% 41% 41% 52%
Golden brown #996515 60% 40% 8% 36° 76% 34% 83% 60%
Golden poppy #FCC200 99% 76% 0% 46° 100% 49% 100% 99%
Golden yellow #FFDF00 100% 87% 0% 52° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Goldenrod #DAA520 85% 65% 13% 43° 74% 49% 85% 85%
Granny Smith Apple #A8E4A0 66% 89% 63% 113° 56% 76% 30% 89%
Grape #6F2DA8 42% 18% 66% 270° 58% 42% 73% 66%
Gray-asparagus #465945 27% 35% 27% 117° 13% 31% 22% 35%
Gray-blue #8C92AC 55% 57% 67% 229° 16% 61% 19% 68%
Green (color wheel) (X11 green) #00FF00 0% 100% 0% 120° 100% 50% 100% 100% Lime
Green (Crayola) #1CAC78 11% 67% 47% 159° 72% 39% 72% 78%
Green (HTML/CSS color) #008000 0% 50% 0% 120° 100% 25% 100% 50% Green
Green (Munsell) #00A877 0% 66% 47% 163° 100% 33% 100% 66%
Green (NCS) #009F6B 0% 62% 42% 160° 100% 31% 100% 62%
Green (pigment) #00A550 0% 65% 31% 149° 100% 32% 100% 65%
Green (RYB) #66B032 40% 69% 20% 95° 56% 44% 72% 69%
Green-yellow #ADFF2F 68% 100% 18% 84° 100% 59% 82% 100%
Grullo #A99A86 66% 60% 53% 34° 17% 59% 21% 66%
Guppie green #00FF7F 0% 100% 50% 150° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Halayà úbe #663854 40% 22% 33% 278° 30% 31% 12% 37%
Han blue #446CCF 27% 42% 81% 223° 59% 54% 67% 81%
Han purple #5218FA 32% 9% 98% 255° 96% 54% 90% 98%
List of colors 82
Hansa yellow #E9D66B 91% 84% 42% 51° 74% 67% 54% 91%
Harvest gold #DA9100 85% 57% 0% 40° 100% 43% 100% 86%
Heart Gold #808000 50% 50% 0% 43° 100% 25% 100% 25%
Heliotrope #DF73FF 87% 45% 100% 286° 100% 73% 55% 100%
Hollywood cerise #F400A1 96% 0% 63% 320° 100% 48% 100% 96%
Honeydew #F0FFF0 94% 100% 94% 150° 100% 97% 97% 97%
Honolulu blue #006DB0 0% 43% 69% 203° 100% 35% 100% 69%
Hooker's green #49796B 29% 47% 42% 163° 25% 38% 40% 48%
Hot magenta #FF1DCE 100% 11% 81% 313° 100% 56% 89% 100%
Hot pink #FF69B4 100% 41% 71% 330° 100% 71% 59% 100%
Hunter green #355E3B 21% 37% 23% 129° 28% 29% 44% 37%
Iceberg #71A6D2 44% 65% 82% 207° 52% 63% 46% 82%
Icterine #FCF75E 99% 97% 37% 58° 96% 68% 63% 99%
Illuminating Emerald #319177 19% 57% 47% 164° 50% 38% 66% 57%
Imperial #602F6B 38% 18% 42% 289° 39% 30% 56% 42%
Imperial blue #002395 0% 14% 58% 226° 100% 29% 100% 58%
Imperial purple #66023C 40% 1% 24% 325° 96% 20% 98% 40%
Imperial red #ED2939 93% 16% 22% 355° 85% 55% 83% 93%
Inchworm #B2EC5D 70% 93% 36% 84° 79% 65% 61% 93%
Indian red #CD5C5C 80% 36% 36% 0° 53% 58% 52% 75%
Indian yellow #E3A857 89% 66% 34% 35° 71% 62% 62% 89%
Indigo (dye) #00416A 0% 25% 42% 203° 100% 21% 100% 42%
Indigo (web) #4B0082 29% 0% 51% 275° 100% 26% 100% 50%
International Klein Blue #002FA7 0% 18% 65% 223° 100% 33% 100% 65%
International orange (aerospace) #FF4F00 100% 31% 0% 19° 100% 50% 100% 100%
International orange (Golden Gate #C0362C 75% 21% 17% 4° 63% 46% 77% 75%
Bridge)
Iris #5A4FCF 35% 31% 81% 245° 57% 56% 62% 81%
Irresistible #B3446C 70% 27% 42% 338° 45% 48% 62% 70%
Italian sky blue #B2FFFF 70% 100% 100% 180° 100% 85% 30% 100%
Japanese indigo #264348 15% 26% 28% 189° 31% 22% 47% 28%
List of colors 83
Japanese violet #5B3256 36% 20% 34% 307° 29% 28% 45% 36%
Jasmine #F8DE7E 97% 87% 49% 47° 90% 73% 49% 97%
Jasper #D73B3E 84% 23% 24% 359° 66% 54% 73% 84%
Jazzberry jam #A50B5E 65% 4% 37% 322° 88% 35% 90% 47%
Jelly Bean #DA614E 85% 38% 31% 8° 65% 58% 64% 86%
June bud #BDDA57 74% 85% 34% 80° 64% 60% 75% 85%
Jungle green #29AB87 16% 67% 53% 163° 61% 42% 76% 67%
Kelly green #4CBB17 30% 73% 9% 101° 78% 41% 88% 73%
Kenyan copper #7C1C05 49% 11% 2% 12° 92% 25% 96% 49%
Keppel #3AB09E 23% 69% 62% 171° 50% 46% 67% 69%
Khaki (HTML/CSS) (Khaki) #C3B091 76% 69% 57% 37° 29% 67% 26% 76%
Khaki (X11) (Light khaki) #F0E68C 94% 90% 55% 54° 77% 75% 42% 94%
Kobi #E79FC4 91% 62% 77% 329° 60% 77% 31% 91%
La Salle Green #087830 3% 47% 19% 141° 88% 25% 93% 47%
Languid lavender #D6CADD 84% 79% 87% 270° 22% 83% 17% 82%
Lapis lazuli #26619C 15% 38% 61% 210° 61% 38% 76% 61%
Laser Lemon #FFFF66 100% 100% 40% 60° 100% 70% 60% 100%
Laurel green #A9BA9D 66% 73% 62% 95° 17% 67% 16% 73%
Lavender (floral) #B57EDC 71% 49% 86% 275° 57% 68% 43% 86%
Lavender (web) #E6E6FA 90% 90% 98% 245° 67% 94% 8% 98%
Lavender blue #CCCCFF 80% 80% 100% 240° 100% 90% 20% 100%
Lavender blush #FFF0F5 100% 94% 96% 340° 100% 97% 6% 100%
Lavender gray #C4C3D0 77% 76% 82% 245° 12% 79% 6% 82%
Lavender indigo #9457EB 58% 34% 92% 265° 79% 63% 63% 92%
Lavender magenta #EE82EE 93% 51% 93% 300° 76% 72% 45% 93%
Lavender mist #E6E6FA 90% 90% 98% 240° 67% 94% 8% 98%
Lavender pink #FBAED2 98% 68% 82% 332° 91% 83% 31% 98%
Lavender purple #967BB6 59% 48% 71% 267° 29% 60% 32% 71%
Lavender rose #FBA0E3 98% 63% 89% 316° 92% 81% 36% 98%
Lawn green #7CFC00 49% 99% 0% 90° 100% 49% 98% 48%
Lemon chiffon #FFFACD 100% 98% 80% 54° 100% 90% 20% 100%
Lemon curry #CCA01D 80% 63% 11% 45° 75% 46% 86% 80%
Lemon glacier #FDFF00 99% 100% 0% 60° 100% 50% 100% 100%
List of colors 84
Lemon lime #E3FF00 89% 100% 0% 44° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Lemon meringue #F6EABE 96% 92% 75% 47° 76% 86% 23% 97%
Lemon yellow #FFF44F 100% 96% 31% 56° 100% 66% 69% 100%
Light apricot #FDD5B1 99% 84% 69% 30° 95% 84% 22% 89%
Light blue #ADD8E6 68% 85% 90% 194° 53% 79% 24% 90%
Light brown #B5651D 71% 40% 11% 28° 72% 41% 84% 71%
Light carmine pink #E66771 90% 40% 44% 350° 72% 65% 70% 80%
Light coral #F08080 94% 50% 50% 0° 79% 72% 50% 100%
Light cornflower blue #93CCEA 58% 80% 92% 201° 67% 75% 37% 92%
Light crimson #F56991 96% 41% 57% 343° 88% 69% 57% 96%
Light cyan #E0FFFF 88% 100% 100% 180° 100% 94% 12% 100%
Light fuchsia pink #F984EF 98% 52% 94% 300° 91% 75% 27% 94%
Light goldenrod yellow #FAFAD2 98% 98% 82% 60° 80% 90% 16% 98%
Light green #90EE90 56% 93% 56% 120° 73% 75% 39% 93%
Light khaki #F0E68C 94% 90% 55% 54° 77% 75% 42% 94%
Light medium orchid #D39BCB 83% 61% 80% 309° 39% 72% 27% 83%
Light moss green #ADDFAD 68% 78% 68% 135° 20% 73% 20% 87%
Light orchid #E6A8D7 90% 66% 84% 315° 55% 78% 27% 90%
Light pastel purple #B19CD9 69% 61% 85% 261° 45% 73% 28% 85%
Light pink #FFB6C1 100% 71% 76% 351° 100% 86% 100% 86%
Light red ochre #E97451 91% 45% 32% 14° 78% 62% 65% 91%
Light salmon #FFA07A 100% 63% 48% 14° 100% 74% 62% 100%
Light salmon pink #FF9999 100% 60% 60% 0° 100% 80% 40% 100%
Light sea green #20B2AA 13% 70% 67% 175° 70% 41% 40% 75%
Light sky blue #87CEFA 53% 81% 98% 203° 92% 76% 46% 98%
Light slate gray #778899 47% 53% 60% 210° 14% 53% 22% 60%
Light steel blue #B0C4DE 69% 77% 87% 214° 41% 78% 21% 87%
Light taupe #B38B6D 70% 55% 43% 26° 32% 56% 39% 70%
Light Thulian pink #E68FAC 90% 56% 67% 330° 64% 73% 72% 94%
Light yellow #FFFFE0 100% 100% 88% 60° 100% 94% 7% 100%
Lilac #C8A2C8 78% 64% 78% 300° 26% 71% 19% 78%
Lime (color wheel) #BFFF00 75% 100% 0% 75° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Lime (web) (X11 green) #00FF00 0% 100% 0% 120° 100% 50% 100% 100% Lime
Lime green #32CD32 20% 80% 20% 120° 61% 50% 67% 40%
Lincoln green #195905 10% 35% 2% 106° 89% 18% 94% 35%
Lion #C19A6B 76% 60% 42% 33° 41% 59% 45% 76%
Little boy blue #6CA0DC 42% 63% 86% 212° 62% 64% 51% 86%
Magenta #FF00FF 100% 0% 100% 300° 100% 50% 100% 100% Fuchsia
Magenta (Crayola) #FF55A3 100% 33% 64% 332° 100% 67% 67% 100%
Magenta (dye) #CA1F7B 79% 12% 48% 326° 73% 46% 90% 79%
Magenta (Pantone) #D0417E 82% 25% 49% 334° 60% 54% 69% 82%
Magenta (process) #FF0090 100% 0% 56% 326° 100% 50% 100% 100%
Magic mint #AAF0D1 67% 94% 82% 150° 70% 80% 84% 80%
Magnolia #F8F4FF 97% 96% 100% 247° 100% 98% 94% 92%
Maize #FBEC5D 98% 93% 36% 54° 95% 68% 63% 98%
Majorelle Blue #6050DC 38% 31% 86% 247° 67% 59% 67% 59%
Manatee #979AAA 59% 60% 67% 231° 10% 63% 11% 67%
Mango Tango #FF8243 100% 51% 26% 20° 100% 63% 74% 100%
Mantis #74C365 45% 76% 40% 110° 44% 58% 48% 77%
Mardi Gras #880085 53% 0% 54% 301° 100% 27% 100% 53%
Maroon (Crayola) #C32148 76% 13% 28% 345° 71% 45% 75% 38%
Maroon (X11) #B03060 69% 19% 38% 333° 57% 44% 65% 42%
Mauve #E0B0FF 88% 69% 100% 276° 100% 85% 31% 100%
Mauve taupe #915F6D 57% 37% 43% 285° 21% 47% 37% 54%
Mauvelous #EF98AA 94% 60% 67% 348° 73% 77% 37% 94%
Maya blue #73C2FB 45% 76% 98% 210° 94% 72% 96% 87%
Meat brown #E5B73B 90% 72% 23% 44° 77% 56% 74% 90%
Medium aquamarine #66DDAA 40% 87% 67% 154° 64% 63% 54% 87%
Medium candy apple red #E2062C 89% 2% 17% 350° 95% 46% 97% 89%
Medium carmine #AF4035 69% 25% 21% 5° 54% 45% 69% 68%
Medium champagne #F3E5AB 95% 90% 67% 48° 75% 81% 30% 95%
Medium electric blue #035096 1% 31% 59% 180° 96% 30% 30% 60%
Medium jungle green #1C352D 11% 21% 18% 120° 31% 16% 20% 20%
Medium lavender magenta #DDA0DD 87% 63% 87% 200° 47% 75% 28% 87%
Medium orchid #BA55D3 73% 33% 83% 288° 59% 58% 60% 83%
Medium Persian blue #0067A5 0% 40% 65% 248° 100% 32% 75% 48%
Medium purple #9370DB 58% 44% 86% 270° 60% 65% 68% 72%
List of colors 86
Medium red-violet #BB3385 73% 20% 52% 322° 57% 47% 79% 83%
Medium ruby #AA4069 67% 25% 41% 337° 45% 46% 62% 67%
Medium sea green #3CB371 24% 70% 44% 150° 50% 47% 42% 30%
Medium sky blue #80DAEB 50% 85% 92% 190° 73% 71% 46% 92%
Medium slate blue #7B68EE 48% 41% 93% 249° 80% 67% 56% 93%
Medium spring bud #C9DC87 79% 86% 53% 80° 55% 70% 70% 80%
Medium spring green #00FA9A 0% 98% 60% 150° 100% 49% 97% 97%
Medium taupe #674C47 40% 30% 28% 9° 18% 34% 31% 40%
Medium turquoise #48D1CC 28% 82% 80% 175° 60% 55% 55% 50%
Medium Tuscan red #79443B 47% 27% 23% 9° 34% 35% 51% 53%
Medium vermilion #D9603B 85% 38% 23% 14° 68% 54% 73% 85%
Medium violet-red #C71585 78% 8% 52% 322° 81% 43% 89% 78%
Mellow apricot #F8B878 97% 72% 47% 30° 90% 72% 52% 97%
Mellow yellow #F8DE7E 97% 87% 49% 47° 90% 73% 49% 97%
Metallic Seaweed #0A7E8C 3% 49% 55% 186° 89% 29% 94% 55%
Metallic Sunburst #9C7C38 61% 49% 22% 41° 47% 42% 64% 61%
Mexican pink #E4007C 89% 0% 49% 327° 100% 45% 100% 89%
Midnight blue #191970 10% 10% 44% 240° 64% 27% 78% 44%
Midnight green (eagle green) #004953 0% 29% 33% 187° 100% 16% 100% 33%
Midori #E3F988 89% 98% 53% 72° 90% 76% 45% 98%
Mikado yellow #FFC40C 100% 77% 5% 45° 100% 52% 95% 100%
Mint #3EB489 24% 71% 54% 158° 49% 48% 66% 71%
Mint cream #F5FFFA 96% 100% 98% 150° 100% 98% 4% 100%
Mint green #98FF98 60% 100% 60% 140° 100% 80% 40% 100%
Misty rose #FFE4E1 100% 89% 88% 6° 100% 94% 12% 100%
Moccasin #FAEBD7 98% 92% 84% 34° 78% 91% 14% 98%
Mode beige #967117 59% 44% 9% 43° 73% 34% 85% 59%
Moonstone blue #73A9C2 45% 66% 76% 199° 39% 61% 41% 76%
Moss green #8A9A5B 54% 60% 36% 75° 26% 48% 41% 60%
Mountain Meadow #30BA8F 19% 73% 56% 161° 59% 46% 74% 73%
Mountbatten pink #997A8D 60% 48% 55% 323° 13% 54% 20% 60%
MSU Green #18453B 9% 27% 23% 167° 48% 18% 65% 27%
Mughal green #306030 19% 38% 19% 120° 33% 28% 50% 38%
Mulberry #C54B8C 77% 29% 55% 285° 51% 53% 67% 70%
Mustard #FFDB58 100% 86% 35% 47° 100% 67% 65% 100%
Myrtle green #317873 19% 47% 45% 176° 42% 33% 59% 47%
For the continuation of the list of colors, please go to List of colors: N-Z#Colors in alphabetical order N-Z.
List of colors 87
Colors by shade
White
White is a balanced combination of all the colors of the visible light spectrum, or of a pair of complementary colors,
or of three or more colors, such as additive primary colors. It isn't a neutral or achromatic (without color) color, like
black and gray.
Pink
Pink is a tint of red, created by adding some white.
Red
Red is any of a number of similar colors evoked by light, consisting predominantly of the longest wavelengths
discernible by the human eye, in the wavelength range of roughly 625–750 nm. It is considered one of the additive
primary colors.
Orange
Orange is the color in the visible spectrum between red and yellow with a wavelength around 585 – 620 nm. In the
HSV color space, it has a hue of around 30°.
Brown
Brown colors are dark or muted shades of reds, oranges, and yellows on the RGB and CMYK color schemes. In
practice, browns are created by mixing two complementary colors from the RYB color scheme (combining all three
primary colors). In theory, such combinations should produce black, but produce brown because most commercially
available blue pigments tend to be comparatively weaker; the stronger red and yellow colors prevail, thus creating
the following tones.
Yellow
Yellow is the color of light with wavelengths predominately in the range of roughly 570–580 nm. In the HSV color
space, it has a hue of around 60°. It is considered one of the subtractive primary colors.
Gray
Achromatic grays are colors between black and white with no hue. Chromatic grays are achromatic grays mixed with
warm hues such as orange (warm grays) or cool hues such as azure (cool grays). This gray color template includes
both achromatic and chromatic grays.
List of colors 88
Green
Green is a color, the perception of which is evoked by light having a spectrum dominated by energy with a
wavelength of roughly 520–570 nm. It is considered one of the additive primary colors.
Cyan
Cyan is any of the colors in the blue-green range of the visible spectrum, i.e., between approximately 520 and
420 nm. It is considered one of the subtractive primary colors.
Blue
Blue is a color, the perception of which is evoked by light having a spectrum dominated by energy with a
wavelength of roughly 440–490 nm. It is considered one of the additive primary colors.
Violet
Violet is any of the colors the perception of which is evoked by light having a spectrum dominated by energy with a
wavelength of roughly 380–450 nm. Tones of violet tending towards the blue are called indigo. Purple colors are
colors that are various blends of violet and blue light with red light.
Web colors
These are the 16 colors which were once deemed "safe" for use in web pages, as they were displayed consistently
across many models of computer monitors. With modern technology, this particular set of colors has become less
relevant.
References
[1] Raggett, Dave (8 April 2002). "Dave Raggett's Introduction to CSS" (http:/ / www. w3. org/ MarkUp/ Guide/ Style). World Wide Web
Consortium. . Retrieved 9 December 2010.
• Frery, A. C.; Melo, C. A. S. & Fernandes, R. C. (13 October 2000). "Web-based Interactive Dynamics for Color
Models Learning" (http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/73504470/abstract). Color Research and
Application 25 (6): 435–441. doi:10.1002/1520-6378(200012)25:6<435::AID-COL8>3.0.CO;2-J. Retrieved
2009-03-15.
External links
• color-book.org (http://color-book.org) Color encyclopedia and tools
• CSS Colour Chart (http://colour.pro/CSS-Colour-Chart.htm)
• Name Of Color (http://name-of-color.com). Online tool to transform color codes between RGB and HSV and
find the closest solid color.
Web colors 89
Web colors
Web colors are colors used in designing web pages, and the methods for describing and specifying those colors.
Colors may be specified as an RGB triplet or in hexadecimal format (a hex triplet). They may also be specified
according to their common English names in some cases. Often a color tool or other graphics software is used to
generate color values. Hexadecimal color codes begin with a number sign (#).[1][2] A color is specified according to
the intensity of its red, green and blue components, each represented by eight bits. Thus, there are 24 bits used to
specify a web color, and 16,777,216 colors that may be so specified.
The first versions of Mosaic and Netscape Navigator used the X11 color names as the basis for their color lists, as
both started as X Window System applications.[3]
Web colors have an unambiguous colorimetric definition, sRGB, which relates the chromaticities of a particular
phosphor set, a given transfer curve, adaptive whitepoint, and viewing conditions.[4] These have been chosen to be
similar to many real-world monitors and viewing conditions, so that even without color management rendering is
fairly close to the specified values. However, user agents vary in the fidelity with which they represent the specified
colors. More advanced user agents use color management to provide better color fidelity; this is particularly
important for Web-to-print applications.
Hex triplet
A hex triplet is a six-digit, three-byte hexadecimal number used in HTML, CSS, SVG, and other computing
applications, to represent colors. The bytes represent the red, green and blue components of the color. One byte
represents a number in the range 00 to FF (in hexadecimal notation), or 0 to 255 in decimal notation. This represents
the least (0) to the most (255) intensity of each of the color components. Thus web colors specify colors in the
Truecolor (24-bit RGB) color scheme. The hex triplet is formed by concatenating three bytes in hexadecimal
notation, in the following order:
Byte 1: red value (color type red)
Byte 2: green value (color type green)
Byte 3: blue value (color type blue)
For example, consider the color where the red/green/blue values are decimal numbers: red=36, green=104, blue=160
(a greyish-blue color). The decimal numbers 36, 104 and 160 are equivalent to the hexadecimal numbers 24, 68 and
A0 respectively. The hex triplet is obtained by concatenating the 6 hexadecimal digits together, 2468A0 in this
example.
Note that if any one of the three color values is less than 16 (decimal) or 10 (hex), it must be represented with a
leading zero so that the triplet always has exactly six digits. For example, the decimal triplet 4, 8, 16 would be
represented by the hex digits 04, 08, 10, forming the hex triplet 040810.
The number of colors that can be represented by this system is 256 × 256 × 256 (2563, or 256 cubed) = 16,777,216.
Web colors 90
The three-digit form is described in the CSS specification, not in HTML. As a result, the three-digit form in an
attribute other than "style" is not interpreted as a valid color in some browsers.
This shorthand form reduces the palette to 4,096 colors, equivalent of 12-bit color as opposed to 24-bit color using
the whole six-digit form (16,777,216 colors). However, this limitation is often sufficient for text based documents.
Yellow #FFFFF0 100% 100% 0% 60° 100% 50% 100% 100% 14 (yellow)
Olive #8080F0 50% 50% 0% 60° 100% 25% 100% 50% 6 (brown)
Lime #F0FFF0 0% 100% 0% 120° 100% 50% 100% 100% 10 (high green); green
Green #F080F0 0% 50% 0% 120° 100% 25% 100% 50% 2 (low green)
Aqua #F0FFFF 0% 100% 100% 180° 100% 50% 100% 100% 11 (high cyan); cyan
Teal #F08080 0% 50% 50% 180° 100% 25% 100% 50% 3 (low cyan)
Blue #F0F0FF 0% 0% 100% 240° 100% 50% 100% 100% 9 (high blue)
Navy #F0F080 0% 0% 50% 240° 100% 25% 100% 50% 1 (low blue)
Web colors 91
Fuchsia #FFF0FF 100% 0% 100% 300° 100% 50% 100% 100% 13 (high magenta); magenta
Purple #80F080 50% 0% 50% 300° 100% 25% 100% 50% 5 (low magenta)
These 16 were labelled as sRGB and included in the HTML 3.0 specification, which noted they were "the standard
16 colors supported with the Windows VGA palette."[10]
Pink colors
Pink FF C0 CB 255 192 203
Red colors
LightSalmon FF A0 7A 255 160 122
Orange colors
OrangeRed FF 45 00 255 69 0
Yellow colors
Yellow FF FF 00 255 255 0
Brown colors
Cornsilk FF F8 DC 255 248 220
Green colors
DarkOliveGreen 55 6B 2F 85 107 47
Cyan colors
MediumAquamarine 66 CD AA 102 205 170
Blue colors
LightSteelBlue B0 C4 DE 176 196 222
Purple colors
Lavender E6 E6 FA 230 230 250
White/Gray/Black colors
White FF FF FF 255 255 255
Web-safe colors
At one time many computer displays were only capable of displaying 256 colors. These may be dictated by the
hardware or changeable by a "color table". When a color is found (e.g., in an image) that is not one available, a
different one has to be used. This can done by either using the closest color, which greatly speeds up the load time,
or by using dithering, which results in more accurate results, but takes a longer to load due to the complex
calculations.
There were various attempts to make a "standard" color palette. A set of colors was needed that could be shown
without dithering on 256-color displays; the number 216 was chosen partly because computer operating systems
customarily reserved sixteen to twenty colors for their own use; it was also selected because it allows exactly six
equally-spaced shades of red, green, and blue (6 × 6 × 6 = 216), each from 00 to FF (including both limits).
The list of colors is often presented as if it has special properties that render them immune to dithering. In fact, on
256-color displays applications can set a palette of any selection of colors that they choose, dithering the rest. These
colors were chosen specifically because they matched the palettes selected by the then leading browser applications.
Fortunately, there were not radically different palettes in use in different popular browsers.
"Web-safe" colors had a flaw in that, on systems such as X11 where the palette is shared between applications,
smaller color cubes (5×5×5 or 4×4×4) were often allocated by browsers—thus, the "web safe" colors would actually
dither on such systems. Better results were obtained by providing an image with a larger range of colors and
Web colors 96
allowing the browser to quantize the color space if needed, rather than suffer the quality loss of a double
quantization.
As of 2011, personal computers typically[14] have 24-bit (TrueColor) and the use of "web-safe" colors has fallen into
practical disuse. Even mobile devices have at least 16-bit color, driven by the inclusion of cameras on cellphones.
The "web-safe" colors do not all have standard names, but each can be specified by an RGB triplet: each component
(red, green, and blue) takes one of the six values from the following table (out of the 256 possible values available
for each component in full 24-bit color).
0 00 0 0
3 33 51 0.2
6 66 102 0.4
9 99 153 0.6
F or (15) FF 255 1
The following table shows all of the "web-safe" colors, underlining the really-safe colors. (One shortcoming of the
web-safe palette is its poor selection of light background colors.) The intensities at the low end of the range,
especially the two darkest, are often hard to distinguish.
Color table
In the table below, each color code listed is a shorthand for the RGB value; for example, code 609 is equivalent to
RGB code 102-0-153 or HEX code #660099.[15]
Web-Safe Colors
*000* 300 600 900 C00 *F00*
CSS colors
The Cascading Style Sheets language defines the same number of named colors as the HTML 4 spec, namely the 16
listed previously. Additionally, CSS 2.1 adds the 'orange' color name to the list[17]:
Web colors 98
CSS 2, SVG and CSS 2.1 also allow web authors to use so-called system colors, which are color names whose values
are taken from the operating system, for example, picking the operating system's highlighted text color, or the
background color for tooltip controls. This enables web authors to style their content in line with the operating
system of the user agent.[18] The CSS3 color module has deprecated the use of system colors in favor of CSS3 UI
System Appearance property,[19][20] which itself was subsequently dropped from CSS3.[21]
The developing CSS3 specification will also introduce HSL color space values to style sheets:
/* RGB model */
p { color: #F00 } /* #rgb */
p { color: #FF0000 } /* #rrggbb */
p { color: rgb(255, 0, 0) } /* integer range 0 - 255 */
p { color: rgb(100%, 0%, 0%) } /* float range 0.0% - 100.0% */
Accessibility
Some browsers and devices do not support colors. For these blind and colorblind users, Web content depending on
colors can be unusable or difficult to use.
Either no colors should be specified (to invoke the browser's default colors), or both the background and all
foreground colors (primarily the colors of plain text, unvisited links, hovered links, active links, and visited links)
should be specified to avoid black on black or white on white effects.[22]
Web colors 99
References
[1] Niederst Robbins, Jennifer. Web Design in a Nutshell, p. 103.
[2] York, Richard. Beginning CSS, pp. 71–72.
[3] Guide to Graphics (http:/ / www. splus. com/ support/ splus80win/ graphics. pdf). SP LUS, splus.com. Page 13.
[4] Digital Color Imaging Handbook By Gaurav Sharma. ISBN 0-8493-0900-X
[5] CSS3 color module (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ css3-color/ #rgb-color)
[6] RGB to Hexadecimal Color Converter (http:/ / www. telacommunications. com/ nutshell/ rgbform. htm)
[7] Color Converter Tool (http:/ / www. colorhexa. com/ )
[8] List of Web Safe Colors with conversions (http:/ / hex-code. com/ web-safe-colors)
[9] HTML 4.01 Specification section 6.5 "Colors" (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ REC-html40/ types. html#h-6. 5)
[10] HTML 3.2 Specification "The BODY element" (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ REC-html32#body)
[11] Public discussion on SVG mailing list Re: color names in SVG-1.0 conflict with /usr/lib/X11/rgb.txt (http:/ / lists. w3. org/ Archives/ Public/
www-svg/ 2002Apr/ 0052. html)
[12] W3C TR CSS3 Color Module, SVG color keywords (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ css3-color/ #svg-color)
[13] W3C TR SVG 1.0, recognized color keyword names (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ SVG/ types. html#ColorKeywords)
[14] Browser Display Statistics (http:/ / www. w3schools. com/ browsers/ browsers_display. asp)
[15] #660099 Color Information (http:/ / www. colorhexa. com/ 660099)
[16] Death of the Websafe Color Palette? (http:/ / www. physics. ohio-state. edu/ ~wilkins/ color/ websafecolors. html)
[17] "CSS 2.1 Specification: Syntax and basic data types: Colors" (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ CSS21/ syndata. html#color-units). 2009-09-08. .
Retrieved 2009-12-21.
[18] User interface - System colors (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ CSS21/ ui. html#system-colors)
[19] CSS3 Color Module - CSS2 System Colors (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ css3-color/ #css-system)
[20] CSS3 Basic User Interface Module, W3C Candidate Recommendation 11 May 2004: System Appearance (http:/ / www. w3. org/ TR/ 2004/
CR-css3-ui-20040511/ #system)
[21] CSS Basic User Interface Module Level 3 (CSS3 UI), W3C Working Draft 17 January 2012: List of substantial changes (http:/ / www. w3.
org/ TR/ css3-ui/ #changes-list), "System Appearance has been dropped, including appearance values & property, and system fonts / extension
of the ‘font’ property shorthand."
[22] If You Pick One Color, Pick Them All (http:/ / www. w3. org/ QA/ Tips/ color)
External links
• CSS2.1 Color Specification (http://www.w3.org/TR/CSS21/syndata.html#color-units)
• Web colors (http://www.dmoz.org/Computers/Graphics/Web/Colors/) at the Open Directory Project
Article Sources and Contributors 100
Color space Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=523636462 Contributors: 166.70.2.xxx, 28421u2232nfenfcenc, AUllrich, Aadnk, Aboalbiss, Aditsu, Adoniscik,
AgentPeppermint, Alan Peakall, AlbertBickford, Alue, Arthena, Asc99c, Ashishbhatnagar72, Aubrey Jaffer, Aursani, Avsa, Beetstra, Benandorsqueaks, Bevo, Bobo192, BorgQueen, Branko,
Brianski, Brona, CALR, CesarB, Chris the speller, Chrisc666, Chrumps, Colordoc, Conversion script, Cpesacreta, Cxw, Cy21, Cyp, Damian Yerrick, DariusMonsef, Darrien, Dave3457,
David1217, Denelson83, Dkroll2, Dmurphy, DocWatson42, Dominus, Dr.K., Dwandelt, FishSpeaker, Fredrik, Funandtrvl, Grendelkhan, Gutza, Hellisp, Heron, Hyacinth, ISteve, IronGargoyle,
Jacobolus, Jay, Jhenderson777, Jim1138, Jonathan Webley, Josef Meixner, Kasper Hviid, Lambtron, Life of Riley, LittleDan, Lovibond, Marluxia.Kyoshu, Mfc, Mindmatrix, Misst r, Moeron,
NekoDaemon, Neparis, Noldoaran, Normankoren, Notinasnaid, Odo1982, Orderud, PAR, Poccil, Rebornsoldier, Retired username, Rhebus, Rich Farmbrough, Richie, Rjwilmsi, Robartsd,
Rythie, Sam Hocevar, SarahPalmerson, Sardanaphalus, Seabhcan, Seattlenow, Sellyme, SharkD, Skarebo, Sluzzelin, Smyth, SocratesJedi, Sparkit, Stephanej, Sterrys, Suruena, Sven271, Ta bu shi
da yu, Tletnes, Twirligig, Uaxuctum, Umofomia, Wapcaplet, Zundark, Ὁ οἶστρος, 147 anonymous edits
Color theory Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=535283173 Contributors: A. B., ABF, Aboalbiss, Addesso, Adoniscik, Adubz2, Ahoerstemeier, Ajonlime, Alemily,
Alexander110, Andrewpmk, Annaversace, Anneyh, Annicedda, Anonim.one, Antandrus, AnubisGFX, Arda Xi, Arnonel, Artseeee, Atlant, Awingfi, Beetstra, BenB4, Benjis, Bigslicksk,
BirdValiant, Bobo192, Booyabazooka, BrokenSegue, Btipling, Burkejwilmore, Caltas, Capt. James T. Kirk, Charles Matthews, ChestRockwell, Chimin 07, Chowa001, Chris.urs-o,
Christopherwillard, CoyneT, Cp111, CrazyTalk, Ctachme, DVD R W, Dain Quentin Gore, Danhash, DanielRigal, Darth Mike, Dave Runger, Daxxter, Dekimasu, Dekuntz, DerHexer, Diabloblue,
DickSummerfield, Dicklyon, Dkroll2, Doktor Wilhelm, Donarreiskoffer, Doodle77, Dougweller, Dozen, Dramartistic, DreamGuy, Dwandelt, Długosz, East718, Edgepedia, Elfguy, Elockid,
Enochlau, EoGuy, Eugene-elgato, Flffy'd, Flying sheep, Fyeguy, Gandalf61, Gary King, Georgia guy, Gerbrant, Giraffedata, Glane23, Glenn, Grick, Grunt, Guioc, Gurch, HaeB, Happysailor,
Hippolyte, Hmains, Holek, Hristo.Hr, Husond, IRP, Iain99, Iamaneyeblinkernoseblower, Imroy, InvertRect, Izalithium, J.delanoy, JForget, JHMM13, JaGa, Jachin, Jacksonj04, Jacobolus,
Jeffro77, Jerknob, Jhenderson777, Jjron, Jmc wiki, Johnpenner, Johnrpenner, Joneddyking, Jonpro, Jossi, Joyous!, Juansidious, Julesd, JustinHagstrom, K9k8, Kazvorpal, Kneale,
KnowledgeOfSelf, Krabstarr, Krystaleen, Lcguang, Lexicon, LogicalDash, Lucaswilkins, Luk, Lusitana, Lysdexia, MER-C, MPerel, Macevoy, Magioladitis, Magister Mathematicae, Magmi,
MarcLevoy, Marsam18 uwgb, Mas Ahmad, MattGiuca, Meaghan, Memami, MetricSuperstar, Michael Hardy, MonteShaffer, Mr.woisard, Mrballistic, Mtd2006, Mud4t, Mysid, Mzhao, Navstar,
Article Sources and Contributors 101
Nbarth, Nehrams2020, NekoDaemon, Netoholic, Newton2, Nilfanion, Nishkid64, Nixeagle, Nn123645, Noon, OMenda, OTB, Omicronpersei8, Oosoom, Ost316, Outriggr, Oxymoron83, Paliku,
Paranomia, Patu999, Paula Clare, Phgao, Philip Trueman, Piano non troppo, PierreCA22, Piet Delport, Poccil, Quadell, Quendus, Quiddity, Quinsareth, Rafti Institute, Rangek, Ray Trygstad,
Rdsmith4, RekishiEJ, RexNL, Rezdave, Rich Farmbrough, Richard cocks, Rick7425, Rrburke, Rune.welsh, Samowen828, Scarlet Lioness, Sceptre, Secrecy611, Sengpheakdey, Seraphim,
Shandris, Shanes, Sharcho, SharkD, Shehal, SimonP, Smartcookie1596, Smilies 101, Some Wiki Editor, Sonjaaa, Spongefrog, Stephan Leeds, Stevertigo, Stib, Strait, SummerWithMorons, TKD,
Tango, Tcncv, Techman224, TheBilly, Thehelpfulone, Tide rolls, Titoxd, Tobias Bergemann, Torgo, TrashLock, UltraMagnus, VMS Mosaic, Waldir, Walter Dufresne, Wine Guy, Wknight94,
Wood Thrush, Wyatt915, X-Fi6, Zeh, Zenao1, Zhatt, Zinjixmaggir, Zoler, Саша Стефановић, 573 anonymous edits
Additive color Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=533721835 Contributors: 2D, AVarchaeologist, Aboalbiss, Bb3cxv, Bryan Derksen, Captain Virtue, Carnildo, CindySteve,
Courcelles, Daniel G., Deracination, Dicklyon, Dkroll2, Dkrolls, Dominus Vobisdu, Dudered, Erimaxbau, GediminasStankevicius, Giants27, Graeme Bartlett, Gurch, Heyzeuss, Incnis Mrsi,
Itzuvit, JDG, Jacobolus, Janke, Jhenderson777, Khazar, Loadmaster, MarcLevoy, Marokwitz, MattTM, Mav, Mbreht, Michael Hardy, Mormegil, Mxn, Nono64, Ost316, Pengo, Peter Isotalo,
Pko, Robbak, Ronhjones, Ruud Koot, Ryoung122, Smb1001, Spet17, Thingg, Thorseth, Thunderbird2, Timberframe, Tjmayerinsf, Toh, VMS Mosaic, Velps, Wyatt915, 49 anonymous edits
Subtractive color Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=529807784 Contributors: 21655, AVarchaeologist, Alansohn, AppuruPan, Beetstra, Bennetto, Bongwarrior,
Booyabazooka, Bsroiaadn, Chickenflicker, Chinasaur, CoyneT, Craig Butz, David-Sarah Hopwood, Delirium, Derek Chong, Dicklyon, Dkroll2, Dkrolls, Dreambeaver, Dysprosia, Erimaxbau,
Erudecorp, Fivestrokes, Gaius Cornelius, Glenn L, Gobeirne, God of War, Gtg204y, Heyzeuss, J.delanoy, Jacobolus, Janke, Jasynnash2, Jhenderson777, Kissiepoo102684, Knotnic, MarcLevoy,
MattTM, Michael Hardy, Mild Bill Hiccup, MisfitToys, Mormegil, Mtodorov 69, Mxn, Newone, NickBush24, Notinasnaid, Ntennis, OMenda, OlEnglish, Reds2010, Scepia, SharkD, Shaunv123,
Shehal, Smack, Stack, Stib, That Guy, From That Show!, Thorseth, Tjmayerinsf, Unfree, VMS Mosaic, Verdy p, Versus22, Wknight94, Wyken Seagrave, Xiong Chiamiov, 93 anonymous edits
Color mixing Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=533031635 Contributors: Akrabbim, Bha100710, ChrisGualtieri, Doozer, Erimaxbau, Excirial, Lithoderm, MSGJ, Moe
Epsilon, Nuvitauy07, RomaC, SF007, Shinjin, Spinningspark, Tad Lincoln, TheBFG, VMS Mosaic, Wyatt915, 25 anonymous edits
Primary color Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=533815928 Contributors: 1exec1, 28421u2232nfenfcenc, 48states, AEMoreira042281, Acroterion, Addesso, Adoniscik,
AgentPeppermint, Ahoerstemeier, Aitrea, AkashBedi12, Alan_D, Alansohn, Alchav, Andrewmc123, Arch dude, Army1987, Arteitle, Atrizu, Avant Guard, AxelBoldt, AxiomShell, BabyD1994,
Beano, Beland, Ben Ben, BenRG, Bgwhite, Bmju, Brianhill, Bryan Derksen, Bryno, Butko, CapitalR, Captain Virtue, Carioca, CaseyPenk, Chack Jadson, Chewie, Chinasaur, Chris the speller,
Chuunen Baka, Citizen Premier, Cmichael, Cometstyles, Connormah, Conversion script, Crd721, Crowsnest, Cruccone, Cult of the Sacred Or nge, Cxw, DHN, Damian Yerrick, Darth Panda,
Daveoh, Davidmcb64, DeadEyeArrow, Deon, Dicklyon, Dkroll2, Doyleb23, DrVenture, Drc79, Dreadstar, Dysepsion, ENeville, Ellywa, Enviroboy, Epbr123, Erdal Ronahi, Fastilysock, Fer408,
Ferengi, Feudonym, Fists, Freelance Intellectual, Funandtrvl, Gemsling, Georgia guy, Gerge125, Glenn L, Glossando, Graham87, GreggEdwards, Gurch, HN45, Hadal, Haham hanuka, Hayabusa
future, Heron, Hpugh123, I*Rok*U*Dont, InternetMeme, Iridescent, J.delanoy, J04n, JDspeeder1, JForget, Jab843, Jackol, Jacobolus, Javawizard, Javert, Jeremy.usman33, Jh12, Jhenderson777,
Jim.henderson, Jmmmmmm, Jossian, Joyonicity, JunCTionS, Katalaveno, Kathartic, Kazvorpal, Keraunos, Kickyandfun, KimvdLinde, Koobmeej, Krystaleen, Kuru, L Kensington, La goutte de
pluie, Lacain247, Laura S, LeaveSleaves, LedgendGamer, Lee Daniel Crocker, LiDaobing, Lucaswilkins, Luna Santin, MER-C, MFNickster, Maggiewoo, Magioladitis, Mandarax, Manway,
Matta96, Max Ijzersteen, Meighan, Melonhead, Mentifisto, Mike902, MikeLynch, Mjad, Mjjj, Moeron, Monster boy1, Mqduck, Mrob27, Myria, Mysid, Nahum Reduta, NekoDaemon,
Neodymion, Nick Number, Niteowlneils, Nopira, Northgrove, Nuno Tavares, Ocean Shores, Ohnjaynb, Onhm, Orphan Wiki, Ost316, PaleAqua, Paulhiphop, PhilKnight, Pi is 3.14159,
Pinethicket, Porridgebowl, Quota, Ram-Man, Raven1977, Rhobite, RingWars2007, Rjwilmsi, Rockgirl745, Roosterrulez, Sarenne, Seaphoto, Shaheenjim, Shoeofdeath, Sicklounge,
Singularitarian, SkerHawx, Stale2000, Steve3849, Stratadrake, Supercoop, Sycthos, Tabletop, Tactik, The Original Wildbear, The Thing That Should Not Be, TheMidnighters, TheSuave, Thelb4,
Themissinglint, Tide rolls, ToastyKen, TransporterMan, Triskaideka, Trjumpet, Tsemii, TyA, UberScienceNerd, VMS Mosaic, VRBones, Velps, VictorianMutant, Vssun, Vzbs34, Wapcaplet,
Waveguy, West.andrew.g, Whatthehell123, White Shadows, Widr, Wiikipedian, WikHead, Wsvlqc, YUL89YYZ, Yekrats, Ziphon, आशीष भटनागर, 512 anonymous edits
Colorfulness Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=528151532 Contributors: 2001:4898:0:FFF:0:5EFE:AC13:901C, Aboalbiss, Adelpine, Adoniscik, Anbu121, Arcadian, Aude,
Casey56, Conscious, CoyneT, DA3N, Dicklyon, Duoduoduo, Ettrig, Frietjes, Fryed-peach, Green Heart 1985, HappyLogolover2011, Interiot, Jacobolus, Keraunos, Kevdave, Lefty, MeekSaffron,
Michael Hardy, MisterSheik, Morecambe1, Natebw, Neilbeach, Night Gyr, Nikai, Ohnoitsjamie, PBS, Paul August, Peter G Werner, Pinethicket, Radagast83, Rjwilmsi, Rmhermen, Roke,
Semolina Pilchard 67, Sims2uni, SmileToday, Sonjaaa, Specious, Spitzak, Srleffler, Svick, Tagishsimon, TallNapoleon, Trevj, Tsiaojian lee, VMS Mosaic, VivaEmilyDavies, Waldir, Wapcaplet,
Welsh, Wiki Raja, Wmhawth, Ylai, 56 anonymous edits
Dichromatism Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=469821596 Contributors: Dmmaus, Eidako, Finog, Fotaun, FrankFE, Jfdwolff, Krefts, Mild Bill Hiccup, Natevw, Pengo,
Rjwilmsi, Scentoni, Srleffler, Woohookitty, 11 anonymous edits
Hue Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=532283716 Contributors: (3ucky(3all, 2DD2, AED, Abdull, Aboalbiss, AugPi, Auntof6, Austinbilleci110, BazookaJoe, Bogey97, CL,
Catinbunny, ChazYork, Chinasaur, Chipcoi1089, Creidieki, Crohnie, Cytex, DHN, Darth Panda, Dbfirs, Deb, DemonThing, Denelson83, Dicklyon, Discospinster, Dyaka, ENeville, Eshmo,
Fanghong, Forelisevn, Fraggle81, FrozenMan, Fryed-peach, GandalfDaGraay, Gogo Dodo, Graham87, Guðsþegn, Hanoipeacetour, Hans Dunkelberg, Hart37, I AM POOTIS, JHunterJ,
Jacobolus, JayJay, Jd4v15, Jheiv, Jimp, Joelr31, JohnManuel, Jppellet, Keraunos, Kingoftheswag, Krystaleen, Lateshift555, Lefty, LittleHow, Loren.wilton, Lovibond, Lulu noodle93,
Lumpbucket, MJ94, Magioladitis, Magnus Manske, Maksim-e, Materialscientist, Mav, Mbubel, Mmh, Mormegil, NOrbeck, Nbarth, NekoDaemon, Neutrality, Nono64, Northumbrian, Olivier,
Ost316, Oxygene123, P. B. Mann, PJV, Pak21, Passengerpigeon, Paweł Ziemian, Picaroon, Pietaster, Prohlep, Rabidtommy, RandomXYZb, Reaper Eternal, RedAndr, Regenspaziergang, RitKill,
Rjwilmsi, Rocket71048576, Rogper, Roke, Runefrost, Ryanmcdaniel, S h i v a (Visnu), SMC, Sander123, Semperveritas, Sgkay, Srleffler, SteinbDJ, Stevertigo, Suran.c, The Thing That Should
Not Be, TheGiantHogweed, ThingXYZ, Thorseth, Tkgd2007, Tumblingsky, VMS Mosaic, Vanished User 8a9b4725f8376, Vardion, VernoWhitney, Wapcaplet, Webclient101, Wernervb, Widr,
Zecora98, Érico Júnior Wouters, ༆, 146 anonymous edits
Tints and shades Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=526002515 Contributors: 23funnel23, Aboalbiss, Adelpine, Alansohn, Avoided, Bertrand Bellet, ContinueWithCaution,
Denispir, Dhynbaa, Download, Grey Maiden, Impy4ever, Incnis Mrsi, Jacobolus, James.Denholm, Jeff G., Jk2q3jrklse, Keraunos, Madijazz2, Malkinann, Martinship, Meco, Mr. Vernon,
Rjeng2000, Robofish, Tgeairn, VMS Mosaic, Xanzzibar, පසිඳු කාවින්ද, 60 anonymous edits
Lightness Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=488695931 Contributors: Aboalbiss, Adelpine, Adoniscik, Chris the speller, Cpoynton, Dicklyon, Drbreznjev, Fatheroftebride,
Fryed-peach, Jacobolus, Jauhienij, Just plain Bill, Keraunos, Kevdave, KlappCK, McSush, Mzajac, Neparis, Ost316, P.B. Pilhet, Pegship, Rich Farmbrough, Ru dagon, SharkD, Srleffler,
Staticshakedown, Svick, The Random Editor, 20 anonymous edits
Opponent process Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=533413851 Contributors: Adoniscik, Ancheta Wis, Anglo-Araneophilus, Benandorsqueaks, Bluemoose, Chinze, Delldot,
Dicklyon, Duoduoduo, Everyking, False vacuum, Forbsey, Geoemyda, Georgia guy, Ghinessk, Incnis Mrsi, Jacobolus, Jesserjames, Johnrpenner, KSmrq, Kcordina, Ketiltrout, Loren.wilton,
MWAK, Magiciandude, Magioladitis, Michael Hardy, Mijelliott, NekoDaemon, Nono64, Poodle slyr, Quibik, Rjwilmsi, Sean.hoyland, Spooky, Staticshakedown, Uaxuctum, Ugncreative
Usergname, Uncle G, VMS Mosaic, Waiting4beckett, Wragge, Wyatt915, 55 anonymous edits
Impossible colors Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=513614931 Contributors: AaRH, Aaron Kauppi, Acebulf, Anthony Appleyard, Auric, Cate, Chipmunkdavis, Chris
Capoccia, Crazymonkey1123 public, Da5nsy, Dasanjos, Davidhorman, Duoduoduo, Extoad, Gioto, Goustien, Itchesavvy, JMatthews, Jonkerz, Jpgordon, Koavf, Lova Falk, Metallaxis, Michael
Hardy, Mikechen, Mmoople, MrBurns, NisseSthlm, OrangeDog, Rhubbarb, Spinningspark, Svick, Tom Piantanida, Tony Mach, VMS Mosaic, Vroo, Wanderer32, Welshie, WryVendor,
Wyatt915, XP1, Ysangkok, Zuchinni one, 34 anonymous edits
Color vision Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=533532095 Contributors: 1993kid, 90 Auto, A314268, AED, Abeg92, Aboalbiss, Acalamari, Adoniscik, AdvCentral,
AgentPeppermint, AlanUS, Angela, Anihl, Aoleson, Aranae, Arcadian, Archer7, Arthena, Atarr, Atlant, Azcolvin429, B jonas, BIGELLOW, Batintherain, Beland, Belovedfreak, BenFrantzDale,
BenRG, Bmju, Bobo192, Bookandcoffee, Britonamission, C A Morris, C.Fred, Calmer Waters, Caribant, CasualVisitor, Cgingold, Chris the speller, Codeye, Crystallina, Cyrius, D, D3z,
Daltonsgirl, Daqu, Delldot, Dicklyon, Dkroll2, Donarreiskoffer, Dpv, Drag-5, Drj11, Duoduoduo, DynamoDegsy, Dysmorodrepanis, Ec5618, Edgeuction, Edison, Efodix, Ego White Tray,
Emperorbma, Enisbayramoglu, Ericmelse, Feitosa-santana, Fred Hsu, Gadfium, Gene Nygaard, Georgia guy, Giftlite, Graft, Guidod, Guillep2k, Guyburns, HallucigeniaUK, Hankwang,
Harryboyles, Hatster301, Headbomb, Hgold, Hunt9, IceKarma, Imagine1989, Iridescent, Jackhynes, Jacobolus, Janke, Jerry, Jhenderson777, Jim.henderson, Joe Decker, Johnkarp, Johnor, Jonas
August, Jonverve, Joriki, JukeJohn, JunCTionS, K kisses, Kazvorpal, Keegan, Keraunos, Kinema, KoenB, Krystaleen, LAX, Last Avenue, Lcguang, Lepidoptera, LilHelpa, Little Mountain 5,
LittleHow, Logical Fuzz, Lotje, LtPowers, Lucasilver, Magioladitis, Magister Mathematicae, MartinPoulter, Marxercise, McGeddon, Medtopic, Mfwitten, MichaelMcGuffin, Micromaster,
Minesweeper, Moonksy29, Munawar6, Nakon, Natrij, Nbarth, NekoDaemon, Nicholashunter, NifCurator1, Njaard, Noetica, NotAnonymous0, Notinasnaid, Nuvitauy07, O.Koslowski, Olivier
Hammam, Orderud, Oscabat, Ost316, Owen, OwenX, PAR, PatrickFisher, Persian Poet Gal, Picus viridis, Pinethicket, Pmineault, Pmronchi, Preeti.sambi, Psheno, Psychron1, QuentinUK, Quota,
RDBrown, ReallyNiceGuy, Rebornsoldier, Rechlin, Renatops, Rich Farmbrough, Richard001, Richerman, Rjwilmsi, Robertg9, SF007, Sanya3, Sayeth, Sbharris, Scwlong, Seabhcan,
Sean.hoyland, Seglea, Selket, Shadowjams, SharkD, Shepaado, SiriusB, Sjöðar, Skatebiker, Smack, Smith609, Sonalprasad, Spigget, Srleffler, StevenBjerke, Suffusion of Yellow, SvenAERTS,
Tabletop, Tagal, TaintedMustard, Talon Artaine, Tckma, The Thing That Should Not Be, TheMindsEye, Thorseth, Threetwoone, Titodutta, Titoxd, Tom Lougheed, Tom harrison, Tyler, Usien6,
VMS Mosaic, Vanessaezekowitz, Vaughan Pratt, Voidxor, Vsmith, Wandell, Washington00, Wernervb, Willhsmit, Wotnow, X-Fi6, YSSYguy, Zarnivop, Zink Dawg, 265 anonymous edits
Visual perception Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=530970814 Contributors: (, A314268, A4, AED, Abqwildcat, Accuruss, Adrian.benko, Alexander Maier, Alexandrov,
Alexis.rodet, Anatoly IVANOV, Ancheta Wis, Andre Engels, Andreamubi, Andreas Kaufmann, Andross52, Angela, Anir1uph, Antony-22, Applesnnbananas, Arcadian, Arfgab, ArglebargleIV,
Argumentum ornithologicum, Ashwin73, Audiovideo, AwamerT, BD2412, Bad Romance, Beland, BenFrantzDale, Bencherlite, Bensaccount, Bentogoa, BirgitteSB, Blue520, Bmju, Boing! said
Article Sources and Contributors 102
Zebedee, Boulaur, Brandon, Bstephens393, CP\M, CanadianLinuxUser, Cgingold, Cleancleaner, Cogpsych, Colin, Cooper24, Cralize, CzarNick, Danger, Darrenhusted, David Latapie,
David-Sarah Hopwood, Dbfirs, DerHexer, Diberri, Dilcoe, Dolfrog, Dontaskme, Dpv, Duja, Duncan, Dycedarg, Ec5618, EdH, Eekerz, Eijiaj80, EinderiheN, Eliyak, Emariatos, Everyking,
Factfinderz, Famousdog, Fastfactchecker, Fenton1234, FlyHigh, Fratrep, FreplySpang, Furrykef, Gary King, Geeoharee, Geraldfird, Giftlite, Gilsatron, Gioto, Glenn, Gludwiczak, Graft,
GreenLocust, Greg.collver, Grutness, Gunza, Gurch, H2g2bob, Halmstad, Hans-Werner34, Hazel77, Hibernian, HieronymusGuinevere, Historychecker, Hordaland, I dream of horses, IRP,
Immunize, Infinitejpower, Insanity Incarnate, Intangir, Intromission, Ixfd64, J04n, Jacobolus, Jagged 85, Janviermichelle, Jarble, Jescamilla4048, Jess523s, Jj1236, Joe yeeha, Johnkarp, Jtkiefer,
JudahH, Jvstone, KYN, Kathy usui, Kfsung, LEMEN, Landroving Linguist, Lars Washington, Leranedo, Lizaahle, LjL, Lochaber, Looie496, Lova Falk, Lussmu, Mac, MajorVariola, Mario1337,
MarkSutton, MarnetteD, Martin Kozák, MartinPoulter, MarylandArtLover, MassimoAr, Mattisse, Matusz, McSly, Mdd, Medtopic, Meegs, Mentifisto, Merovingian, Michael Hardy,
Midnightcomm, Mild Bill Hiccup, Mindmatrix, Mjr Armstrong, Mlessard, Modulatum, Mogism, MortimerCat, MrOllie, MuZemike, Mukadderat, Mwarren us, Naddy, NawlinWiki, Neostinker,
Nerrolken, Neuropsychology, Never give in, Nick, Nk, Nneonneo, Noetica, Novem Linguae, Nuvitauy07, ObfuscatePenguin, PLA y Grande Covián, Parkjunwung, Patrick, Pedant, Perfect
Proposal, Peter T.S., Peterlin, Petter Trillkott, PhilHibbs, Pizza Puzzle, Pleasantville, Poetaris, Pollinosisss, Poshzombie, Privong, Psychron1, Purielku, RSaunders, RadioElectric, Ragesoss,
Rarara1111, Raudys, Richard001, Rimtect, Roadnottaken, Robertg9, Rokers, RoyBoy, Rryswny, RyanCross, Sacre, Sadi Carnot, Sae Harshberger, Sango123, Sanya3, Sardanaphalus, Sbarthelme,
Sbluen, Scathane, Seabhcan, Seeyou, Selket, Sephiroth BCR, Sfan00 IMG, Snoyes, SoCal, Solipsist, Soundray, Sparkleyone, Speed8ump, SpeedyGonsales, Spencerk, Srenee91, StealthB, Steve
Pucci, Strasburger, Svick, Swanav, Tango, Tangotango, Template namespace initialisation script, Tevildo, Tharshikatee, The Anome, The Magnificent Clean-keeper, Thebigone45,
Themfromspace, Thesoxlost, Thetoothpick, Thingg, Thongsftw, Tierlieb, Tobby72, Tony1, Twinsday, Tó campos, Utcursch, Vaughan, Verne Equinox, Vsion, Vssun, Wakebrdkid, Waterfall117,
Werdan7, West.andrew.g, Where next Columbus?, White Trillium, Wildkoala, William M. Connolley, Wolfdog, Woohookitty, Worldbookman, X96lee15, Xevi, Xyoureyes, Yidisheryid, Ylem,
Zephy2034, Περίεργος, 337 anonymous edits
List of colors Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=525745536 Contributors: 041744, 15thWardWestBank, 220 of Borg, 2602:306:CD88:5940:64DB:EA79:CDFD:D2B6,
4twenty42o, AArthur150, AGK, Abb615, Abcdefg987654, Abce2, Ace ETP, Addshore, Adreantruth, Adrey, Aeron Valderrama, Aesopos, Agesilaus II, AgnosticPreachersKid, Ahoerstemeier,
Ahonc, Airplaneman, Aitias, Aksi great, AlanBarrett, Alansohn, Alexkin, Alexzabbey, Allens, Alpha Ralpha Boulevard, Alsandro, Altenmann, Altermike, AmericaIsNumberOne, Anbu121,
Andrea105, Andros 1337, Andux, AngelOfSadness, Angelofdeath275, Anonymous Dissident, Antandrus, Antique Rose, Apparition11, Arch dude, Ardric47, ArglebargleIV, Arlo Barnes,
Arthena, Arxiloxos, Asciic, Asdfjklqazse, Ashishbhatnagar72, Atomicharcoal, Avengingbandit, Avoided, B58PMCSA, BD2412, Backpackadam, Bagatelle, Barrelbabe3737, BarretB, Bart133,
Baseball Bugs, Blue520, Bluedenim, Bluenectarine, Bobo192, Bobrayner, Bongwarrior, Brainsurge33, Bucketsofg, Bumm13, Burndownthedisco, Burntpiecrust, Bus stop, Butros, CUSENZA
Mario, CWii, Cacums, Cait1208, Calmer Waters, Camw, Canterbury Tail, Canthusus, Capedia, Capricorn42, Captain Virtue, Captain panda, Catgut, Cathyahn, Ccson, Centrx, Cerebralpayne,
Cgmusselman, Charles Matthews, Charlottelai, Chaser, Chasingsol, Chienlit, Chris the speller, Christian75, Chuunen Baka, Circeus, Closedmouth, Cnilep, Cntras, ColinBoylett, Collingwood,
Comedy Dan, Cooliesare, Coopdawg7533, Coopkev2, Corington, Corvus cornix, Coryknick, Courcelles, CoyneT, Crazy Boris with a red beard, Creidieki, Crissov, Crocodealer, Crohnie,
CrowzRSA, Cspenc36, Ctachme, Cubs Fan, CyberSkull, Cyberkid ua, D2513850, DARTH SIDIOUS 2, DJ Clayworth, DMacks, DRTllbrg, DVdm, DaDoc540, Daniele Pugliesi, Darrien, Darth
Newdar, David0811, Davidbod, Davish Krail, Gold Five, Dbenbenn, Dbfirs, Dbritnell, Dcljr, DeC, DeWayne, Dekisugi, Denelson83, Denisarona, Deor, Diamond941, Dicklyon, Digfarenough,
DiplomBastler, Dirjarmocksorz, Discospinster, Divide, Djaychela, Docu, Dodo's Conundrum, Donarreiskoffer, Donfbreed, Draco, Drilnoth, Dude1818, Dureo, Dyolf, E2eamon, ESkog,
Eaglemach, Ebyabe, Eeekster, Eequor, Ehrenkater, Elevenzeroone, Emnat, Emrrans, Epbr123, Evaders99, Excirial, Extra999, Fabrizio.sporchia, Falcon8765, FastLizard4, Feinoha, Festyzizzle,
Ffffloyd, Finalius, Fish and karate, FisherQueen, Flewis, Fluffernutter, Fluiday, Foobaz, For An Angel, Fran McCrory, FreplySpang, Frietjes, Friginator, Frigotoni, From Selma to Stonewall,
Funandtrvl, Funnyfarmofdoom, Fëaluinix, GabrielF, Gail, Garnerguy123, Geahanse, Gene Nygaard, Geodog242, George Hernandez, Georgia guy, Glacialfox, Glenn L, Gogo Dodo, Gojistomp,
Gracenotes, GrayFullbuster, Groat, Grunt, Gtrmp, Guoguo12, Gus Polly, Gwernol, HJ Mitchell, Halfway to never, Hammersoft, HappyInGeneral, Happysugarrush, Harkenbane, Hasek is the best,
Hayabusa future, Heatseeker0, Heegoop, Henning Makholm, Hermitage17, Heron, Hghyux, Hmrox, Hongking, Hqb, Hushguppy, Husond, IAmTheCoinMan, IW.HG,
Ididntrecognizethatlastepisode, Igoldste, Ihateakiva, Ijstmariedashton, IlyaHaykinson, Immunize, Imroy, IncognitoErgoSum, Indon, Ionistii, IronGargoyle, ItsZippy, Itsyourmom, IwantNapalm,
Ixfd64, J. Naven, J.delanoy, JForget, JV Smithy, Ja 62, JaGa, Jaberwocky6669, Jack Merridew, Jackol, Jacobolus, Jadesfans, Jagginess, Jak123, Jakew, JamieS93, Jebus0, Jeepday, Jeff G.,
JeremyA, Jeromesyroyal, JesusAddict3791, Jhenderson777, Jimp, Jj.pitrelli, Jklin, Jmlk17, Jncraton, John Anderson, John254, Jojit fb, Joseph Solis in Australia, Josephycc, Jurema Oliveira,
Jusjih, Jusses2, JustAGal, KJBracey, Kafka Liz, Kballigator4, KelleyCook, Kenkku, Keraunos, Kesac, Kevinkor2, Kilo-Lima, Kingpin13, Kingturtle, Kintetsubuffalo, Klausness, Klingac,
Knotwrite, KoG Iceman, Koavf, Kobayashis, Korax1214, Krishvanth, Kruusamägi, Krystaleen, Kudret abi, Kuru, Kwamikagami, Kyle1278, Kylet, Kyorosuke, L Kensington, LAPS, La Pianista,
Lacrymocéphale, Latina Ashburg, Latitude0116, Launchballer, Laura S, Lax4mike, Lchiarav, Leafyplant, Lee J Haywood, Lee15482, Lefty, LemonTwinkle, Lesnail, Liastnir, Libcub, Lights,
Lironos, Locke Cole, Logdick, Lollipopluver415, Loopla, Luffyzors, Luk, LunchBox5181, M0rphzone, MCTales, MPF, Mabdul, MacMan2626, Macy, Maethordaer, Mahewa, Majorly,
Mandarax, Mani1, Marasama, Marauder40, Marek69, MarnetteD, Martarius, Mastershake phd, Mathel, Mathratio, Matt Deres, Mcmillin24, Mdebets, Mean as custard, MelSkunk,
MementoVivere, Meningitis888, Mephistophelian, Merovingian, Michel BUZE, Mike Rosoft, Mike902, Mild Bill Hiccup, Mindmatrix, Minesweeper.007, MiracleMat, Mit-aizama, Mitchazenia,
Mjquinn id, Mlpearc, Modernist, Monsieurdl, Monster boy1, Montana's Defender, Moonriddengirl, Moony103, Morwen, MrDolomite, Mtking, Mxn, My76Strat, Mário, N5iln, NHJG,
NHRHS2010, Naep1, Naggers, Nahum Reduta, Nakon, Nameneko, Naryathegreat, Nascar1996, NawlinWiki, NellieBly, Nemen32, Nepfabcd, Nesnad, Netalarm, Nevin.williams, Nezzadar,
Nguyenthephuc, Nightscream, Nikkikm, Nkocharh, Noah22, Noleander, Norbu, Notinasnaid, Nuberfuzzy, OlEnglish, Oliver202, Oms1005, Onorem, Orangemike, Orphan Wiki, Oxymoron83,
PPWen, PacMan1980, Paepaok, PaleAqua, Pan Camel, ParisianBlade, Patkwok, Pax85, Peterxdeng, Phil Boswell, Phil Bridger, Philip Trueman, Picaroon, Pinethicket, Pip2andahalf, Pjhofmann,
Plastikspork, Poeloq, Polyguy05, Ppanzini, Praefectorian, ProtoFire, Psychonaut, Pufferfish101, Puffin, Puppy132, Pyfan, Qajar, Quantumobserver, QuartierLatin1968, Quiddity, Qxz,
RAZOR1911xSKiDROWotp, RL0919, Radioactive afikomen, Ramaksoud2000, Rambam rashi, Rbanzai, Rdococ, Reaper Eternal, Recognizance, Redfarmer, Redvers, Renzut, Retiono Virginian,
RexNL, Rholton, Rich Farmbrough, Richard-of-Earth, RichardF, Rigadoun, Riggr Mortis, Rikkus, Rjwilmsi, Rlendog, Rlevse, Roberta F., Rochelimit, Roland Richter, RoseCherry, Rowman,
RoyLeban, Rrburke, Rreagan007, Rror, Rsduhamel, Ruby Emo, Ryan Taylor, Ryanmcdaniel, Ryulong, SJP, SRFFGT, Saccerzd, Saimhe, Saturday, Scarce, Scmaruthi, ScottAlanHill, Scribble
Monkey, Secondarywaltz, Secret Saturdays, Shanes, Shazzam32, Shd, Shell Kinney, Shervinemami, Shosetsuka, Shyamthirumalai, Siddhant, Sigma 7, Silivrenion, SilverPhoenix99,
Simtropolitan, Sionus, Skweltch, SkyWalker, Slightsmile, Slon02, Slposey, Smurrayinchester, Socal gal at heart, Some jerk on the Internet, Someoneinmyheadbutit'snotme, Spanglej, Sphilbrick,
Splash, SportsMaster, Squirepants101, Srleffler, Ssbbplayer, Starfarmer, Steven Zhang, Storm Rider, Strand, Strdst grl, SuperHamster, Sven Manguard, Swmnjen3, Synchronism, TAnthony,
THEN WHO WAS PHONE?, TRLIJC19, TUF-KAT, Tajik24, Tassedethe, Tbhotch, TeaDrinker, Tempodivalse, TenPoundHammer, The Banner, The High Fin Sperm Whale, The Midna, The
Missing Piece, The Thing That Should Not Be, The Tom, The editor1, Thecheesykid, Theme97, Thingg, This, that and the other, Thisisborin9, Thisisbossi, Thomas Kelly, Thue, Thumperward,
TiagoTiago, Tide rolls, Tnxman307, ToastyMallows, Tobias Bergemann, Toddst1, Tohd8BohaithuGh1, Tommy2010, Tonkun, Tonywalton, Tree Biting Conspiracy, Trialsanderrors, Trivialist,
Troyheidenberg, True Pagan Warrior, Trusilver, TutterMouse, Tyrol5, UU, Uncle Dick, Usb10, Useight, Utcursch, VMS Mosaic, Valyrd, Vanished user 39948282, Varlaam, Vectro,
Vegaswikian, Velella, Verbalise, Verdy p, VeronicaVericuta, Vicarious, Vishvax, Voxii, WLU, Waldir, Wavelength, Waymorecreative, Wayne Miller, Wayne Slam, Wetman, WiKinny,
Wiikipedian, WikHead, Wiki alf, Wiki13, WikiY, Wikipelli, Wildoneshelper, WillDarlock, Willking1979, Wknight94, Wolfforlife40, Wop67, Woudloper, Wpell, Wtmitchell, Wzwz, Xinyu,
Xkonax, Xyzzy n, Yacht, Yaris678, Yun-Yuuzhan (lost password), Yurymik, ZaferXYZ, Zappa711, Zaui, Zemaniac, Zephyr103, Zhatt, Zoe, Zombie Hunter Smurf, Zondor, ZooFari, Zzyzx11,
Ødipus sic, Саша Стефановић, 金 肅, 3348 anonymous edits
Web colors Source: http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?oldid=533774725 Contributors: 4myself4, A000040, ABF, Aapo Laitinen, AerisMTW, Akuchling, Alan Liefting, AlistairMcMillan,
Alsandro, Altermike, Andrejj, Andretheseal, AndrewWTaylor, Andrewaskew, Andrewpmk, Angelo, Angr, Anonymous Dissident, Apoc2400, Arbitrarily0, Army1987, Arnon Chaffin,
Ashishbhatnagar72, Atomician, AussieLegend, Ayrton Prost, Badgernet, Bewildebeast, Bjankuloski06en, Bloodpack, Blow of Light, Bluedenim, Bobo192, Boing! said Zebedee, BorgQueen,
Btipling, CUSENZA Mario, CattleGirl, Causesobad, CesarB, Chris the speller, ChristTrekker, Christopher Parham, CloudStrife, Codingmasters, CoyneT, Crissov, Crohnie, Ctachme, CyberSkull,
Cyp, Damian Yerrick, Damieng, Danthorpe2002, Daveswagon, David Biddulph, David Gerard, David Kernow, Dbreakey, Den fjättrade ankan, Denelson83, Denisutku, Deryck Chan, Dicklyon,
Discospinster, Donarreiskoffer, DougsTech*com, Dxhtml, Dysprosia, Ed g2s, Edgepedia, Edokter, Efa, ElTchanggo, Eleveneleven, Ellywa, Elphion, Escape Orbit, Excirial, EyeSerene, Fabartus,
Fanghong, Farsouth, FastLizard4, Fieldday-sunday, Finbarr Saunders, Flashmorbid, Florian Blaschke, Fluffernutter, Franci.kapel, Fresheneesz, FuriousFreddy, Furrykef, G Allegre, Gelo71, Geni,
Giftlite, GizmoRay, Glenn L, Gobonobo, Googlere, Gordeonbleu, Groovybreadstick, Gus Polly, Gwillhickers, Hadal, HaeB, Hang Li Po, HappyLogolover2011, Hardwick, Hawky, Heegoop,
Henry Flower, HereToHelp, Heron, Hightilidie, Hja, Hopper-blackrose, Hossen27, Howard the Duck, Hu12, Husond, Hyad, Hydrargyrum, ILovePlankton, Ian Fieggen, Iketsi, Indefatigable,
Ipatrol, Itsyourmom, Ixfd64, J.delanoy, JECompton, JWB, Jamelan, JanineR, Janke, Jeromesyroyal, Jfd34, Jfioeawfjdls453, Jhenderson777, Jim1138, Jimp, Jleedev, Jmh, Jmlk17, Jonathan W,
Jonwoolley, Jor, Jsjunkie, Jtneill, Keraunos, Kim Bruning, Kjoonlee, Ksy92003, KyleVD, LOL, La Pianista, Launchballer, Leandrod, Lights, Linguisticsstud, Loadmaster, Logoorange,
Lowellian, Lugia2453, Luk, LunaticBeatnik, M.O.X, Macedonian, Macha Panta, Madpilot, Makandal14, Mani1, Manop, Maquiguy, Martarius, MattGiuca, MaxHund, MeStinkBAD, Menchi,
Mepolypse, MetaManFromTomorrow, Mindmatrix, Mobykanuno, Monster boy1, MonteShaffer, Moondyne, Moopstick, Mouse is back, Mr.Z-man, MrDolomite, MrOllie, Ms2ger, Mulad,
Mun206, Mutinus, NantonosAedui, Nein, NekoDaemon, NewEnglandYankee, Nickshanks, Nigelj, Nikola Smolenski, Nilfanion, Nokya, Notinasnaid, Nwwaew, Oaktwig, OhanaUnited,
Ohnoitsjamie, OlEnglish, Oliver H, Omniplex, PC78, PaleAqua, Patrick, Paul A, Paul August, Pax:Vobiscum, Pejno Simono, Pengo, Perey, Peruvianllama, Phil Boswell, PhilHibbs, Philip
Trueman, Pi is 3.14159, Pipatron, Pixeltoo, Pixi, PizzaMargherita, Pnm, PointedEars, Pomte, PopeButtercockXIV, Power Slave, Prodego, Psychonaut, Quiddity, Quinxorin, R'n'B, Rbucci,
Rebornsoldier, Red Slash, Reddi, Redrose64, Rehnn83, Reisio, Renice, RespekT, Ricardo Cancho Niemietz, Rich Farmbrough, Richard D. LeCour, Richard75, RichardF, Rick Cooper, RobertG,
Robwingfield, Ropers, RoyBoy, Ruakh, Rursus, Salam32, Samw, Scapler, Scjessey, Seb az86556, Secondarywaltz, Seidenstud, Sentience, Serevinus, Shadowjams, Shanes, Shepazu, Signalhead,
Simetrical, SimonDeDanser, Smalljim, Sodaplayer, Sophie, StaticGull, Stephenj642, Stormie823, Strcat, Sumirp, SunCountryGuy01, Superm401, Svick, Swagger28, Syp, Takai003,
TastyPoutine, Tgeairn, The Anome, The Illusive Man, The Mysterious El Willstro, The Thing That Should Not Be, The editor1, Tholly, Tilankar, Timothy Clemans, Trainra, Traxs7, Tristanb,
Turlo Lomon, Twentydragon, UU, Uannis, Ugur Basak, VMS Mosaic, Verdy p, Victor Engel, Vipinhari, W1tgf, WODUP, Waldir, Wapcaplet, Warlordwolf, Wayne InSane (Of RTC), Wayne
Slam, Werdan7, Wertuose, WhatamIdoing, Whytecypress, Wikitonic, XaeL, Xiong, Ybenharim, Yugsdrawkcabeht, Zad68, ZimZalaBim, Zoicon5, Zollerriia, Zundark, 484 anonymous edits
Image Sources, Licenses and Contributors 103
License
Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 3.0 Unported
//creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/